E. Bates, V. Carlsonluden, and I. Bretherton, Perceptual aspects of tool using in infancy, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.3, pp.127-140, 1980.
DOI : 10.1016/S0163-6383(80)80017-8

A. L. Brown, Domain-Specific Principles Affect Learning and Transfer in Children, Cognitive Science, vol.14, pp.107-133, 1990.

M. Carpenter, J. Call, and M. Tomasello, Understanding "Prior Intentions" Enables Two-Year-Olds to Imitatively Learn a Complex Task, Child Development, vol.73, issue.5, pp.1431-1441, 2002.
DOI : 10.1111/1467-8624.00481

Z. Chen and R. S. Siegler, Across the great divide: bridging the gap between understanding of toddlers' and older children's thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, pp.1-96, 2000.

K. Connolly and M. Dalgleish, The emergence of a tool-using skill in infancy., Developmental Psychology, vol.25, issue.6, pp.894-912, 1989.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.25.6.894

G. Cisbra and G. Gergely, Social learning and social cognition. The case for pedagogy Processes of Change in Brain and Cognitive Development. Attention and Performance XXI, Munakata, pp.249-274, 2006.

B. Elsner, Infants??? imitation of goal-directed actions: The role of movements and action effects, Acta Psychologica, vol.124, issue.1, pp.44-59, 2007.
DOI : 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.09.006

R. Esseily, J. Nadel, and J. Fagard, Object retrieval through observational learning in 8- to 18-month-old infants, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.33, issue.4, pp.695-699, 2010.
DOI : 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.07.017

S. Hirata, N. Morimura, and C. Houki, How to crack nuts: acquisition process in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) observing a model, Animal Cognition, vol.138, issue.S1, pp.87-101, 2009.
DOI : 10.1007/s10071-009-0275-3

R. Keen, The Development of Problem Solving in Young Children: A Critical Cognitive Skill, Annual Review of Psychology, vol.62, issue.1, pp.1-21, 2011.
DOI : 10.1146/annurev.psych.031809.130730

I. Kiraly, The effect of the model???s presence and of negative evidence on infants??? selective imitation, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol.102, issue.1, pp.14-25, 2009.
DOI : 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.06.003

J. J. Lockman, A Perception-Action Perspective on Tool Use Development, Child Development, vol.71, issue.1, pp.137-144, 2000.
DOI : 10.1111/1467-8624.00127

M. E. Mccarty, R. K. Clifton, and R. R. Collard, Problem solving in infancy: The emergence of an action plan., Developmental Psychology, vol.35, issue.4, pp.1091-1101, 1999.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.35.4.1091

M. E. Mccarty, R. K. Clifton, and R. R. Collard, The Beginnings of Tool Use by Infants and Toddlers, Infancy, vol.1, issue.2, pp.233-256, 2001.
DOI : 10.1207/S15327078IN0202_8

M. Nielsen, G. Simcock, and L. Jenkins, The effect of social engagement on 24-month-olds??? imitation from live and televised models, Developmental Science, vol.437, issue.5, pp.722-731, 2008.
DOI : 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00722.x

K. O-'regan, L. Rat-fischer, and J. Fagard, Mechanisms leading to tool use: A longitudinal study in human infants, Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience Conference Abstract: IEEE ICDL- EPIROB 2011, 2011.

J. Piaget, The origins of intelligence in the child, 1952.

J. Provasi, C. D. Dubon, and H. Bloch, Do 9-and 12-month-olds learn means-ends relation by observing? Infant behavior and development, pp.195-213, 2001.

K. D. Sage and D. A. Baldwin, Disentangling the Social and the Pedagogical in Infants' Learning about Tool-Use, Social Development, vol.35, issue.4, pp.825-844, 2011.
DOI : 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011.00624.x

L. Van-leeuwen, A. Smitsman, and C. Van-leeuwen, Affordances, perceptual complexity, and the development of tool use., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.20, issue.1, pp.174-191, 1994.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.20.1.174

T. M. Barrett, E. Traupman, and A. Needham, Infants??? visual anticipation of object structure in grasp planning, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.31, issue.1, pp.1-9, 2008.
DOI : 10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.05.004

L. J. Claxton, M. E. Mccarty, and R. Keen, Self-directed action affects planning in tool-use tasks with toddlers, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.32, issue.2, pp.230-233, 2009.
DOI : 10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.12.004

H. Cochet, M. Jover, and J. Vauclair, Hand preference for pointing gestures and bimanual manipulation around the vocabulary spurt period, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol.110, issue.3, pp.393-407, 2011.
DOI : 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.04.009

D. Corbetta and E. Thelen, Lateral biases and fluctuations in infants' spontaneous arm movements and reaching, Developmental Psychobiology, vol.299, issue.4, pp.237-255, 1999.
DOI : 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199905)34:2<237::AID-DEV1>3.0.CO;2-#

D. M. Corey, M. M. Hurley, and A. L. Foundas, Right and left handedness defined: A multivariate approach using hand preference and hand performance measures, Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychology, and Behavioral Neurology, vol.14, issue.3, pp.144-152, 2001.

K. S. Cornwell, L. J. Harris, and H. E. Fitzgerald, Task effects in the development of hand preference in 9???, 13???, and 20???month???old infant girls, Developmental Neuropsychology, vol.35, issue.1, pp.19-34, 1991.
DOI : 10.1016/0028-3932(75)90067-6

J. F. Coryell and G. F. Michel, How supine postural preferences of infants can contribute toward the development of handedness, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.1, pp.245-257, 1978.
DOI : 10.1016/S0163-6383(78)80036-8

R. F. Cox and A. W. Smitsman, Action planning in young children's tool use, Developmental Science, vol.36, issue.6, pp.628-641, 2006.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.35.3.651

G. Dellatolas, P. T. Bitter, P. Curt, and F. , Evolution of Degree and Direction of Hand Preference in Children: Methodological and Theoretical Issues, Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, vol.8, issue.4, pp.387-399, 1997.
DOI : 10.1111/j.2044-835X.1988.tb01099.x

J. Fagard and C. Lemoine, THE ROLE OF IMITATION IN THE STABILIZATION OF HANDEDNESS DURING INFANCY, Journal of Integrative Neuroscience, vol.05, issue.04, pp.519-533, 2006.
DOI : 10.1142/S0219635206001343

J. Fagard and J. J. Lockman, The effect of task constraints on infants??? (bi)manual strategy for grasping and exploring objects, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.28, issue.3, pp.305-315, 2005.
DOI : 10.1016/j.infbeh.2005.05.005

J. Fagard and A. Streri, Développement des asymétries chez le bébé, Droitiers/Gauchers : des asymétries dans tous les sens, pp.49-70, 2004.

J. Fagard, L. Rat-fischer, and J. K. O-'regan, Comment le bébé accède-t-il à la notion d'outil ? How do infants acquire the concept of tool? Enfance, pp.73-84, 2012.

J. Fagard, Hand preference, communicative gestures, and praxis in infants. Paper presented at CogEvo, 2012.

C. L. Ferre, I. Babik, and G. F. Michel, Development of infant prehension handedness: A longitudinal analysis during the 6- to 14-month age period, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.33, issue.4, pp.492-502, 2010.
DOI : 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.06.002

F. Flament, Coordination et prévalence manuelle chez le nourrisson, Editions du Centre Chapter Principal steps in tool use National de la Recherche Scientifique, 1975.

N. Geschwind and A. M. Galaburda, Cerebral Lateralization, Archives of Neurology, vol.42, issue.5, pp.428-459, 1985.
DOI : 10.1001/archneur.1985.04060050026008

A. Gesell and L. B. Ames, The Development of Handedness, The Pedagogical Seminary and Journal of Genetic Psychology, vol.32, issue.5, pp.155-175, 1947.
DOI : 10.1037/10756-006

P. R. Hawn and L. J. Harris, Hand differences in grasp duration and reaching in two-and fivemonth-old infants, Manual specialization and the developing brain, pp.331-348, 1983.

P. G. Hepper, S. Shahidullah, and R. White, Handedness in the human fetus, Neuropsychologia, vol.29, issue.11, pp.29-1107, 1991.
DOI : 10.1016/0028-3932(91)90080-R

P. G. Hepper, D. L. Wells, and C. Lynch, Prenatal thumb sucking is related to postnatal handedness, Neuropsychologia, vol.43, issue.3, pp.313-315, 2005.
DOI : 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.08.009

W. D. Hopkins and D. M. Rabinowitz, Manual Specialisation and Tool Use in Captive Chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ): The Effect of Unimanual and Bimanual Strategies on Hand Preference, Laterality, vol.2, issue.3, pp.3-4, 1997.
DOI : 10.1080/135765097397486

T. Humle and T. Matsuzawa, Laterality in hand use across four tool-use behaviors among the wild chimpanzees of Bossou, Guinea, West Africa, American Journal of Primatology, vol.36, issue.1, pp.40-48, 2009.
DOI : 10.1002/ajp.20616

A. Jacquet, R. Esseily, D. Rider, and J. Fagard, Handedness for grasping objects and declarative pointing: A longitudinal study, Developmental Psychobiology, vol.96, issue.3, pp.36-46, 2012.
DOI : 10.1002/dev.20572

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01429957

S. H. Johnson-frey, R. Newman-norlund, and S. T. Grafton, A Distributed Left Hemisphere Network Active During Planning of Everyday Tool Use Skills, Cerebral Cortex, vol.15, issue.6, pp.681-695, 2005.
DOI : 10.1093/cercor/bhh169

D. J. Lewkowicz and G. Turkewitz, Influence of hemispheric specialization in sensory processing on reaching in infants: Age and gender related effects., Developmental Psychology, vol.18, issue.2, pp.301-308, 1982.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.18.2.301

E. V. Lonsdorf and W. D. Hopkins, Wild chimpanzees show population-level handedness for tool use, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol.102, issue.35, pp.12634-12638, 2005.
DOI : 10.1073/pnas.0505806102

K. Mackenzie and M. Peters, Handedness, Hand Roles, and Hand Injuries at Work, Journal of Safety Research, vol.31, issue.4, pp.221-227, 2000.
DOI : 10.1016/S0022-4375(00)00041-4

M. E. Mccarty, R. K. Clifton, and R. R. Collard, Problem solving in infancy: The emergence of an action plan., Developmental Psychology, vol.35, issue.4, pp.1091-1101, 1999.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.35.4.1091

M. E. Mccarty, R. K. Clifton, and R. R. Collard, The Beginnings of Tool Use by Infants and Toddlers, Infancy, vol.1, issue.2, pp.233-256, 2001.
DOI : 10.1207/S15327078IN0202_8

C. M. Mccormick and D. M. Maurer, Unimanual hand preferences in 6-month-olds: Consistency and relation to familial-handedness, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.11, issue.1, pp.21-29, 1988.
DOI : 10.1016/S0163-6383(88)80013-4

W. C. Mcgrew and L. F. Marchant, Using the tools at hand: Manual laterality and elementary technology in Cebus spp. and Pan spp, International Journal of Primatology, vol.18, issue.5, pp.787-810, 1997.
DOI : 10.1023/A:1026347913888

K. J. Meador, D. W. Loring, K. Lee, M. Hughes, G. Lee et al., Cerebral lateralization: Relationship of language and ideomotor praxis, Neurology, vol.53, issue.9, pp.53-2028, 1999.
DOI : 10.1212/WNL.53.9.2028

G. F. Michel, M. R. Ovrut, and D. A. Harkins, Hand-use preference for reaching and object manipulation in 6-through 13-month-old infants, Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, vol.111, issue.4, pp.407-427, 1985.

G. F. Michel, A. N. Tyler, C. Ferre, and C. F. Sheu, The manifestation of infant hand-use preferences when reaching for objects during the seven- to thirteen-month age period, Developmental Psychobiology, vol.26, issue.6, pp.436-479, 2006.
DOI : 10.1002/dev.20161

F. Morange and H. Bloch, Lateralization of the Approach Movement and the Prehension Movement in Infants from 4 to 7 Months, Early Development and Parenting, vol.12, issue.2, pp.81-92, 1996.
DOI : 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0917(199606)5:2<81::AID-EDP119>3.0.CO;2-M

K. O-'regan, L. Rat-fischer, and J. Fagard, Mechanisms leading to tool use: A longitudinal study in human infants, 2011.

C. Porac, S. Coren, J. H. Steiger, and P. Duncan, Human laterality: A multidimensional approach., Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie, vol.34, issue.1, pp.91-96, 1980.
DOI : 10.1037/h0081015

D. S. Ramsay, Onset of unimanual handedness in infants, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.3, pp.377-385, 1980.
DOI : 10.1016/S0163-6383(80)80045-2

A. M. Leslie, Some implications of pretense for mechanisms underlying the child's theory of mind, 1988.

A. M. Leslie and S. Keeble, Do six-month-olds perceive causality? Cognition, pp.265-288, 1987.

O. 'regan, J. K. Rat-fischer, L. Fagard, and J. , Mechanisms leading to tool use: A longitudinal study in human infants, Front. Computer Neuroscience Conference Abstract: IEEE ICDL, 2011.

J. Piaget, The origins of intelligence in children (M. Cook, Trans.), 1952.

L. Rat-fischer, J. K. O-'regan, and J. Fagard, The emergence of tool use during the second year of life, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol.113, issue.3, pp.440-446, 2012.
DOI : 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.001

L. Rat-fischer, O. Regan, J. Fagard, and J. , Handedness in infants' tool use, Developmental Psychobiology, vol.33, issue.4, 2012.
DOI : 10.1002/dev.21078

H. M. Richardson, The growth of adaptive behaviour in infants: An experimental study at seven age levels, Genetic Psychology Monographs, XII, pp.195-357, 1932.

G. Rizzolatti and L. Fadiga, The mirror-neuron system and action recognition Higher-order motor disorders: from Neuroanatomy and Neurobiology to Clinical Neurology, 2005.

M. Schlesinger and J. Langer, Infants' developing expectations of possible and impossible tool-use events between ages 8 and 12 months, Developmental Science, vol.2, issue.2, pp.195-205, 1999.
DOI : 10.1111/1467-7687.00068

A. Schlottmann, Perception versus knowledge of cause and effect in children : when seeing is believing, Current directions in psychological science, pp.111-115, 2011.

E. Spelke, K. Breinlinger, J. Macomber, and K. Jacobson, Origins of knowledge., Psychological Review, vol.99, issue.4, pp.605-632, 1992.
DOI : 10.1037/0033-295X.99.4.605

E. Spelke, Preferential looking methods as tools for the study of cognition in infancy Measurement of Audition and Vision in the First Year of Postnatal Life, Ablex, 1985.

&. Sommerville and . Woodward, Pulling out the intentional structure of action: the relation between action processing and action production in infancy, Cognition, vol.95, issue.1, pp.1-30, 2005.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.12.004

I. C. Uzgiris and J. M. Hunt, Assessment in infancy : ordinal scales of psychological development, 1975.

L. Van-leeuwen, A. Smitsman, and C. Van-leeuwen, Affordances, perceptual complexity, and the development of tool use., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.20, issue.1, pp.174-191, 1994.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.20.1.174

C. Von-hofsten, Action in Infancy: A foundation for Cognitive Development Action science: Foundations of an emerging discipline, pp.255-280, 2013.

C. Von-hofsten and L. Rönnqvist, Preparation for grasping an object: A developmental study., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.14, issue.4, pp.610-621, 1988.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.14.4.610

C. Von-hofsten, K. Rosander, E. Bates, V. Carlson-luden, and I. Bretherton, Development of smooth pursuit tracking in young infants. Vision References Perceptual aspects of tool using in infancy, pp.127-140, 1980.

H. Bekkering, A. Wohlschlager, and M. Gattis, Imitation of Gestures in Children is Goal-directed, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, vol.1, issue.1, pp.153-164, 2000.
DOI : 10.1126/science.4012321

F. Bellagamba and M. Tomasello, Re-enacting intended acts: comparing 12- and 18-month-olds, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.22, issue.2, pp.277-282, 1999.
DOI : 10.1016/S0163-6383(99)00002-8

A. L. Brown, Domain-specific principles affect learning and transfer in children. Chapter 5, Observational learning Cognitive Science, vol.14, issue.1, pp.107-133, 1990.

M. Carpenter, N. Akhtar, and M. Tomasello, Fourteen- through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.21, issue.2, pp.315-330, 1998.
DOI : 10.1016/S0163-6383(98)90009-1

M. Carpenter, J. Call, and M. Tomasello, Understanding "Prior Intentions" Enables Two-Year-Olds to Imitatively Learn a Complex Task, Child Development, vol.73, issue.5, pp.1431-1441, 2002.
DOI : 10.1111/1467-8624.00481

M. Carpenter, J. Call, and M. Tomasello, Twelve- and 18-month-olds copy actions in terms of goals, Developmental Science, vol.31, issue.1, pp.13-20, 2005.
DOI : 10.1111/1467-9280.00218

Z. Chen and R. S. Siegler, Across the great divide: bridging the gap between understanding of toddlers' and older children's thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 2000.

L. Claxton, M. E. Mccarty, and R. Keen, Self-directed action affects planning in tool-use tasks with toddlers, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.32, issue.2, pp.230-233, 2009.
DOI : 10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.12.004

K. Connolly and M. Dalgleish, The emergence of a tool-using skill in infancy., Developmental Psychology, vol.25, issue.6, 1989.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.25.6.894

G. Csibra, Goal attribution to inanimate agents by 6.5-month-old infants, Cognition, vol.107, issue.2, pp.705-717, 2008.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.08.001

B. Elsner, Infants??? imitation of goal-directed actions: The role of movements and action effects, Acta Psychologica, vol.124, issue.1, pp.44-59, 2007.
DOI : 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.09.006

B. Elsner and G. Aschersleben, Do I get what you get? Learning about the effects of self-performed and observed actions in infancy, Consciousness and Cognition, vol.12, issue.4, pp.732-751, 2003.
DOI : 10.1016/S1053-8100(03)00073-4

B. Elsner, P. Hauf, and G. Aschersleben, Imitating step by step: A detailed analysis of 9- to 15-month-olds??? reproduction of a three-step action sequence, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.30, issue.2, pp.325-335, 2007.
DOI : 10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.10.001

R. Esseily, J. Nadel, and J. Fagard, Object retrieval through observational learning in 8- to 18-month-old infants, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.33, issue.4, pp.695-699, 2010.
DOI : 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.07.017

J. Fagard and J. Lockman, Change in imitation for object manipulation between 10 and 12 months of age, Developmental Psychobiology, vol.352, issue.9129, pp.90-99, 2010.
DOI : 10.1002/dev.20416

G. Gergely, H. Bekkering, and I. Kiràly, Rational imitation in preverbal infants, 2002.

K. Kamewari, K. Masaharu, K. Takayuki, I. Hiroshi, and H. Kazuo, Six-and-a-halfmonth-old children positively attribute goals to human action and to humanoid-robot motion, Observational learning Cognitive Development, pp.303-320, 2005.

I. Kiraly, The effect of the model???s presence and of negative evidence on infants??? selective imitation, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol.102, issue.1, pp.14-25, 2009.
DOI : 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.06.003

R. Keen, The Development of Problem Solving in Young Children: A Critical Cognitive Skill, Annual Review of Psychology, vol.62, issue.1, pp.1-21, 2011.
DOI : 10.1146/annurev.psych.031809.130730

M. E. Mccarty, R. K. Clifton, and R. R. Collard, The Beginnings of Tool Use by Infants and Toddlers, Infancy, vol.1, issue.2, pp.233-256, 2001.
DOI : 10.1207/S15327078IN0202_8

A. N. Meltzoff, Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children., Developmental Psychology, vol.31, issue.5, pp.31-838, 1995.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.838

A. N. Meltzoff, The ???like me??? framework for recognizing and becoming an intentional agent, Acta Psychologica, vol.124, issue.1, pp.26-43, 2007.
DOI : 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.09.005

K. O-'regan, L. Rat-fischer, and J. Fagard, Mechanisms leading to tool use: a longitudinal study in human infants. Front, Computer Neuroscience Conference Abstract, 2011.

M. Paulus, S. Hunnius, M. Vissers, and H. Bekkering, Imitation in infancy: Rational or motor resonance ? Child Development, pp.1047-1057, 2011.

C. L. Poulson, L. R. Nunes, and S. F. Warren, Imitation in Infancy: A Critical Review, 1989.
DOI : 10.1016/S0065-2407(08)60417-6

L. Rat-fischer, K. O-'regan, and J. Fagard, The emergence of tool-using during the second year of life, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

V. Southgate, C. Chevallier, and G. Csibra, Sensitivity to communicative relevance tells young children what to imitate, Developmental Science, vol.42, issue.2, pp.1013-1019, 2009.
DOI : 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00861.x

V. Southgate, M. H. Johnson, and G. Csibra, Infants attribute goals even to biomechanically impossible actions, Cognition, vol.107, issue.3, pp.1059-1069, 2008.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.10.002

L. Van-leeuwen, A. Smitsman, and C. Van-leeuwen, Affordances, perceptual complexity, and the development of tool use., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.20, issue.1, pp.174-91, 1994.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.20.1.174

P. Willatts, Development of means-end behavior in young infants: Pulling a support to retrieve a distant object., Developmental Psychology, vol.35, issue.3, pp.651-667, 1999.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.35.3.651

A. L. Woodward, Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach, Cognition, vol.69, issue.1, 1998.
DOI : 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00058-4

A. L. Woodward, E. Levan-goldschmidt, and M. B. Stevenson, Infants' ability to distinguish between purposeful and nonpurposeful behaviors. Infant Behavior and Development Action Imitation: The Early Phase of Infancy, References References References Abravanel, Child Development, vol.22, issue.47, pp.145-160, 1976.

S. H. Ambrose, Paleolithic Technology and Human Evolution, Science, vol.291, issue.5509, pp.1748-1753, 2001.
DOI : 10.1126/science.1059487

J. M. Baldwin, A New Factor in Evolution, The American Naturalist, vol.30, issue.354, pp.441-451, 1896.
DOI : 10.1086/276408

K. A. Bard and J. Vauclair, The communicative context of object manipulation in ape and human adult-infant pairs, Journal of Human Evolution, vol.13, issue.2, pp.181-190, 1984.
DOI : 10.1016/S0047-2484(84)80062-7

E. Barrett, E. Davis, and A. Needham, Learning about tools in infancy., Developmental Psychology, vol.43, issue.2, pp.352-368, 2007.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.43.2.352

E. Bates, L. Carnaioni, and V. Volterra, The acquisition of performatives prior to speech, Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, vol.21, pp.205-224, 1975.

E. Bates, V. Carlsonluden, and I. Bretherton, Perceptual aspects of tool using in infancy, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.3, issue.2, pp.127-140, 1980.
DOI : 10.1016/S0163-6383(80)80017-8

H. A. Baumgartner and L. M. Oakes, Investigating the Relation Between Infants' Manual Activity With Objects and Their Perception of Dynamic Events, Infancy, vol.108, issue.6, 2013.
DOI : 10.1111/infa.12009

B. B. Beck, Animal tool behavior, NewYork: Garland, 1980.

V. Bentley-condit and E. O. Smith, Animal tool use: current definitions and an updated comprehensive catalog, Behaviour, vol.147, issue.2, pp.185-221, 2010.
DOI : 10.1163/000579509X12512865686555

S. Biro and A. M. Leslie, Infants? perception of goal-directed actions: development through cue-based bootstrapping, Developmental Science, vol.23, issue.3, pp.379-398, 2007.
DOI : 10.1111/1467-9280.00218

D. F. Björklund, Cognitive development: An overview, Handbook of Developmental Psychology, 2011.

T. Breuer, M. Ndoundou-hockemba, and V. Fishlock, First Observation of Tool Use in Wild Gorillas, PLoS Biology, vol.54, issue.11, pp.2041-2043, 2005.
DOI : 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030380.g002

A. L. Brown, Domain-Specific Principles Affect Learning and Transfer in Children, Cognitive Science, vol.14, issue.1, pp.107-133, 1990.

J. P. Boudreau and E. W. Bushnell, Spilling thoughts: configuring attentional resources in infants??? goal-directed actions, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.23, issue.3-4, pp.3-4, 2000.
DOI : 10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00059-5

E. W. Bushnell and J. P. Boudreau, Exploring and exploiting objects with the hands during infancy, The psychobiology of the hand, pp.144-161, 1998.

E. W. Bushnell, J. Sidman, and A. E. Brugger, Transfer according to the means in human infants: The secret to generative tool-use? Stone knapping: The necessary conditions for a uniquely hominid behaviour, pp.303-317, 2006.

M. Carpenter, J. Call, and M. Tomasello, Understanding "Prior Intentions" Enables Two-Year-Olds to Imitatively Learn a Complex Task, Child Development, vol.73, issue.5, pp.1431-1441, 2002.
DOI : 10.1111/1467-8624.00481

M. Carpenter, N. Akhtar, and M. Tomasello, Fourteen- through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.21, issue.2, pp.315-330, 1998.
DOI : 10.1016/S0163-6383(98)90009-1

M. Carpenter, K. Nagell, and M. Tomasello, Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, pp.1-143, 1998.

Z. Chen and R. S. Siegler, Across the great divide: bridging the gap between understanding of toddlers' and older children's thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, pp.1-96, 2000.

C. K. Cheng and M. A. Schmuckler, Action-perception progress in tool use development, Paper presented at the 15 th Biennial International Conference on Infant Studies, 2008.

S. Chevalier-skolnikoff and J. O. Liska, Tool use by wild and captive elephants, Animal Behaviour, vol.46, issue.2, pp.209-219, 1993.
DOI : 10.1006/anbe.1993.1183

L. J. Claxton, R. Keen, and M. E. Mccarty, Evidence of Motor Planning in Infant Reaching Behavior, Psychological Science, vol.14, issue.4, pp.354-56, 2003.
DOI : 10.1111/1467-9280.24421

L. J. Claxton, M. E. Mccarty, and R. Keen, Self-directed action affects planning in tool-use tasks with toddlers, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.32, issue.2, pp.230-233, 2009.
DOI : 10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.12.004

K. Connolly and M. Dalgleish, The emergence of a tool-using skill in infancy., Developmental Psychology, vol.25, issue.6, pp.894-912, 1989.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.25.6.894

D. Corbetta and K. E. Bojczyk, Infants Return to Two-Handed Reaching When They Are Learning to Walk, Journal of Motor Behavior, vol.35, issue.1, pp.83-95, 2002.
DOI : 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00029-8

D. Corbetta and E. Thelen, The developmental origins of bimanual coordination: A dynamic perspective., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.22, issue.2, pp.502-522, 1996.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.22.2.502

R. F. Cox and A. W. Smitsman, Action planning in young children's tool use, Developmental Science, vol.36, issue.6, pp.628-641, 2006.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.35.3.651

R. F. Cox and A. W. Smitsman, Hand-use perseveration in young children's use of a spoon: a dynamic model, 2006.

R. F. Cox and A. W. Smitsman, The planning of tool-to-object relations in young children, Developmental Psychobiology, vol.72, issue.2, pp.178-186, 2006.
DOI : 10.1002/dev.20117

C. M. Craig and D. N. Lee, Neonatal control of nutritive sucking pressure: evidence for an intrinsic ??-guide, Experimental Brain Research, vol.124, issue.3, pp.371-382, 1999.
DOI : 10.1007/s002210050634

G. Cisbra and G. Gergely, Social learning and social cognition. The case for pedagogy, 2006.

V. Deecke, Tool-use in the brown bear (Ursus arctos), Animal Cognition, vol.57, issue.4, pp.725-730, 2012.
DOI : 10.1007/s10071-012-0475-0

J. Duncan and A. M. Owen, Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands, Trends in Neurosciences, vol.23, issue.10, pp.475-483, 2000.
DOI : 10.1016/S0166-2236(00)01633-7

B. Elsner, Infants??? imitation of goal-directed actions: The role of movements and action effects, Acta Psychologica, vol.124, issue.1, pp.44-59, 2007.
DOI : 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.09.006

N. J. Emery and N. S. Clayton, Tool use and physical cognition in birds and mammals, Current Opinion in Neurobiology, vol.19, issue.1, pp.27-33, 2009.
DOI : 10.1016/j.conb.2009.02.003

R. Esseily, J. Nadel, and J. Fagard, Object retrieval through observational learning in 8- to 18-month-old infants, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.33, issue.4, pp.695-699, 2010.
DOI : 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.07.017

R. Esseily, Observatinal learning of new manual skills in the first two years of life, 2010.

J. Fagard, C. Florean, L. Rat-fischer, and J. K. O-'regan, When do infants understand that the parts of composite objects move together ?

J. Fagard and A. Y. Jacquet, Onset of bimanual coordination and symmetry versus asymmetry of movement, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.12, issue.2, pp.229-236, 1989.
DOI : 10.1016/0163-6383(89)90009-X

J. Fagard and J. J. Lockman, Change in imitation for object manipulation between 10 and 12 months of age, Developmental Psychobiology, vol.352, issue.9129, pp.90-99, 2010.
DOI : 10.1002/dev.20416

J. Fagard and A. Marks, Unimanual and bimanual tasks and the assessment of handedness in toddlers, Developmental Science, vol.3, issue.2, pp.137-147, 2000.
DOI : 10.1111/1467-7687.00107

J. Fagard and A. Pezé, Age Changes in Interlimb Coupling and the Development of Bimanual Coordination, Journal of Motor Behavior, vol.57, issue.2, pp.199-208, 1997.
DOI : 10.1037/0003-066X.45.8.938

J. Fagard, L. Rat-fischer, and J. K. O-'regan, Comment le b??b?? acc??de-t-il ?? la notion d???outil???, Enfance, vol.2012, issue.01, pp.73-84, 2012.
DOI : 10.4074/S0013754512001085

S. A. Gerson and A. L. Woodward, A claw is like my hand: Comparison supports goal analysis in infants, Cognition, vol.122, issue.2, pp.181-192, 2012.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.10.014

A. Gesell and F. L. Ilg, Infant and Child in the Culture Today. The Guidance of Development in Home and Nursery School, 1943.

E. J. Gibson, Exploratory Behavior in the Development of Perceiving, Acting, and the Acquiring of Knowledge, Annual Review of Psychology, vol.39, issue.1, pp.1-41, 1988.
DOI : 10.1146/annurev.ps.39.020188.000245

J. J. Gibson, The senses considered as perceptual systems, 1966.

J. J. Gibson, The ecological approach to perception, 1979.

E. J. Gibson and A. D. Pick, An Ecological Approach to Perceptual Learning and Development patients and healthy participants who have difficulties in tool use, 2000.

J. Goodall, The chimpanzees of Gombe, 1986.

M. L. Greif and A. Needham, The development of human tool use in early life Tool use and causal cognition, pp.51-68, 2011.

A. Grenier, La "motricité libérée" par fixation manuelle de la nuque au cours des premières semaines de la vie. Archives Françaises de Pédiatrie, pp.557-562, 1981.

J. J. Guajardo and A. L. Woodward, Is Agency Skin Deep? Surface Attributes Influence Infants' Sensitivity to Goal-Directed Action, Infancy, vol.11, issue.3, pp.361-384, 2004.
DOI : 10.1207/s15327078in0603_3

F. Guerin, N. Krüger, and D. Kraft, A Survey of the Ontogeny of Tool Use: From Sensorimotor Experience to Planning, IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development, vol.5, issue.1, pp.18-45, 2013.
DOI : 10.1109/TAMD.2012.2209879

S. A. Gerson and A. L. Woodward, A claw is like my hand: Comparison supports goal analysis in infants, Cognition, vol.122, issue.2, pp.181-192, 2012.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.10.014

J. K. Hamlin, E. V. Hallinan, and A. L. Woodward, Do as I do: 7-month-old infants selectively reproduce others??? goals, Developmental Science, vol.17, issue.1), pp.487-494, 2008.
DOI : 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00694.x

M. Hayashi and T. Matsuzawa, Cognitive development in object manipulation by infant chimpanzees, Animal Cognition, vol.6, issue.4, pp.225-233, 2003.
DOI : 10.1007/s10071-003-0185-8

P. G. Hepper, D. L. Wells, and C. Lynch, Prenatal thumb sucking is related to postnatal handedness, Neuropsychologia, vol.43, issue.3, pp.313-315, 2005.
DOI : 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.08.009

S. J. Hespos and R. Baillargeon, Infants' Knowledge About Occlusion and Containment Events: A Surprising Discrepancy, Psychological Science, vol.19, issue.3, pp.141-147, 2001.
DOI : 10.1111/1467-9280.00324

S. J. Hespos and R. Baillargeon, Décalage in infants' reasoning about occlusion and containment events: Converging evidence from action tasks, Cognition, vol.99, pp.34-41, 2006.

G. R. Hunt, Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows, Nature, vol.379, issue.6562, pp.249-251, 1996.
DOI : 10.1038/379249a0

N. Inoue-nakamura and T. Matsuzawa, Development of stone tool use by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)., Journal of Comparative Psychology, vol.111, issue.2, pp.159-173, 1997.
DOI : 10.1037/0735-7036.111.2.159

H. Ishibashi, S. Hihara, and A. Iriki, Acquisition and development of monkey tool-use: behavioral and kinematic analyses, Canadian Journal of Physiology and Pharmacology, vol.78, issue.11, pp.958-966, 2000.
DOI : 10.1139/y00-063

B. A. Kahrs, W. P. Jung, and J. J. Lockman, What is the role of infant banging in the development of tool use?, Experimental Brain Research, vol.38, issue.2, pp.315-320, 2012.
DOI : 10.1007/s00221-012-3062-3

R. Keen, The Development of Problem Solving in Young Children: A Critical Cognitive Skill, Annual Review of Psychology, vol.62, issue.1, pp.1-21, 2011.
DOI : 10.1146/annurev.psych.031809.130730

B. Kenward, C. Schloegl, C. Rutz, A. S. Weir, T. Bugnyar et al., On the evolutionary and ontogenetic origins of tool-oriented behaviour in New Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides), Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, vol.102, issue.4, pp.870-877, 2011.
DOI : 10.1111/j.1095-8312.2011.01613.x

B. Kenward, C. Rutz, A. Weir, and . Kacelnik, Development of tool use in New Caledonian crows: inherited action patterns and social influences, Animal Behaviour, vol.72, issue.6, pp.1329-1343, 2006.
DOI : 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.04.007

M. Kimmerle, L. A. Mick, and G. Michel, Bimanual role-differentiated toy play during infancy, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.18, issue.3, pp.299-307, 1995.
DOI : 10.1016/0163-6383(95)90018-7

I. Kiràly, The effect of the model???s presence and of negative evidence on infants??? selective imitation, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol.102, issue.1, pp.14-25, 2009.
DOI : 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.06.003

W. Köhler, The mentality of apes, 1925.

L. Lefebvre, N. Nicolakakis, and D. Boire, TOOLS AND BRAINS IN BIRDS, Behaviour, vol.139, issue.7, pp.939-973, 2002.
DOI : 10.1163/156853902320387918

L. Legrain, A. Destrebecqz, and W. Gevers, The role of goal-directed behavior in intention attribution in children, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol.112, issue.3, pp.351-359, 2012.
DOI : 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.01.004

J. J. Lockman, A Perception-Action Perspective on Tool Use Development, Child Development, vol.71, issue.1, pp.137-144, 2000.
DOI : 10.1111/1467-8624.00127

T. Mccormack, C. Hoerl, and S. A. Butterfill, Tool use and causal cognition, 2011.

J. Mann, B. L. Sargeant, J. J. Watson-capps, Q. A. Gibson, M. R. Heithaus et al., Why Do Dolphins Carry Sponges?, PLoS ONE, vol.69, issue.12, p.3868, 2008.
DOI : 10.1371/journal.pone.0003868.t001

B. Fruth and G. Hohmann, Nest building behavior in the great apes: the great leap forward?, pp.225-240, 1996.
DOI : 10.1017/CBO9780511752414.019

E. Matheson, A study of problem solving in preschool children, Child Development, vol.2, pp.242-262, 1931.

M. E. Mccarty, R. K. Clifton, and R. R. Collard, Problem solving in infancy: The emergence of an action plan., Developmental Psychology, vol.35, issue.4, pp.1091-1101, 1999.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.35.4.1091

M. E. Mccarty, R. K. Clifton, and R. R. Collard, The Beginnings of Tool Use by Infants and Toddlers, Infancy, vol.1, issue.2, pp.233-256, 2001.
DOI : 10.1207/S15327078IN0202_8

W. Mcgrew, Chimpanzees material culture: Implications for human evolution, 1992.

W. C. Mcgrew, L. F. Marchant, and T. Nishida, Great Ape Societies, 1996.
DOI : 10.1017/CBO9780511752414

A. N. Meltzoff, Infant Imitation and Memory: Nine-Month-Olds in Immediate and Deferred Tests, Child Development, vol.59, issue.1, pp.217-225, 1988.
DOI : 10.2307/1130404

A. N. Meltzoff, Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children., Developmental Psychology, vol.31, issue.5, pp.838-850, 1995.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.838

E. Moro and E. Freundenberg, Der Morosche Umklammerungs-reflex und das Brudzins-kische Nachkenzeichen als Reflexe des Seuglingsalters, 1921.

C. Nabeshima, Y. Kuniyoshi, and M. Lungarella, Adaptive body schema for robotic tool-use, Advanced Robotics, vol.20, issue.10, pp.1105-1126, 2006.
DOI : 10.1163/156855306778522550

K. Nagell, R. S. Olguin, and M. Tomasello, Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens)., Journal of Comparative Psychology, vol.107, issue.2, pp.174-186, 1993.
DOI : 10.1037/0735-7036.107.2.174

M. Nielsen, G. Simcock, and L. Jenkins, The effect of social engagement on 24-month-olds??? imitation from live and televised models, Developmental Science, vol.437, issue.5, pp.722-731, 2008.
DOI : 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00722.x

F. Osiurak, C. Jarry, M. Lesourd, J. Baumard, L. Gall et al., Mechanical problemsolving strategies in left-brain damaged patients and apraxia of tool use, Neuropsychologia, 2013.

E. M. Patterson and J. Mann, Look, no hands! Open peer commentary to Vaesen, K. The cognitive bases of human tool use, Behavioral and brain sciences, vol.35, pp.203-262

J. Piaget, The origins of intelligence in children (M. Cook, Trans.), 1952.

L. Rat-fischer, J. K. O-'regan, and J. Fagard, The emergence of tool use during the second year of life, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, vol.113, issue.3, pp.440-446, 2012.
DOI : 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.06.001

L. Rat-fischer, O. Regan, J. Fagard, and J. , Handedness in infants' tool use, Developmental Psychobiology, vol.33, issue.4, 2012.
DOI : 10.1002/dev.21078

L. Rat-fischer, J. K. O-'regan, and J. Fagard, Visual attention in a means-end task: the case of multiple strings

H. M. Richardson, The growth of adaptive behavior in infants: An experimental study at seven age levels, Genetic Psychology Monographs, XII, pp.195-357, 1932.

K. D. Sage and D. A. Baldwin, Disentangling the Social and the Pedagogical in Infants' Learning about Tool-Use, Social Development, vol.35, issue.4, pp.825-844, 2011.
DOI : 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011.00624.x

M. Schlesinger and J. Langer, Infants' developing expectations of possible and impossible tool-use events between ages 8 and 12 months, Developmental Science, vol.2, issue.2, pp.195-205, 1999.
DOI : 10.1111/1467-7687.00068

M. A. Schmuckler, Perceptual-motor relations in obvious and non-obvious domains: A history and review, Handbook of Developmental Psychology, 2011.

A. M. Seed and R. W. Byrne, Animal Tool-Use, Current Biology, vol.20, issue.23, pp.1032-1039, 2010.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.042

URL : http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.042

L. B. Smith and E. Thelen, Development as a dynamic system, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol.7, issue.8, pp.343-348, 2003.
DOI : 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00156-6

A. W. Smitsman and R. F. Cox, Perseveration in Tool Use: A Window for Understanding the Dynamics of the Action-Selection Process, Infancy, vol.11, issue.1, pp.249-269, 2008.
DOI : 10.1080/15250000802004379

A. W. Smitsman and D. Corbetta, Action in Infancy - Perspectives, Concepts, and Challenges, Blackwell Handbook of Infant development, 2010.
DOI : 10.1002/9781444327564.ch5

&. Sommerville and . Woodward, Pulling out the intentional structure of action: the relation between action processing and action production in infancy, Cognition, vol.95, issue.1, pp.1-30, 2005.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.12.004

E. Somogyi, C. Ara, L. Rat-fischer, J. K. O-'regan, and J. Fagard, Infants benefit more from Function familiarization than object familiarization when learning to use a tool

E. Spelke, K. Breinlinger, J. Macomber, and K. Jacobson, Origins of knowledge., Psychological Review, vol.99, issue.4, pp.605-632, 1992.
DOI : 10.1037/0033-295X.99.4.605

E. Spelke and G. Van-de-walle, Perceiving and reasonning about objects: insights from infants, Spatial representation, pp.132-161, 1993.

J. P. Spencer, B. Vereijken, F. J. Diedrich, and E. Thelen, Posture and the emergence of manual skills, Developmental Science, vol.3, issue.2, pp.216-233, 2000.
DOI : 10.1111/1467-7687.00115

. St, R. Amant, and T. Horton, Revisiting the definition of animal tool use, Animal Behaviour, vol.75, pp.1199-1208, 2008.

G. Stoet and L. H. Snyder, The role of executive control in tool use. Open peer commentary to Vaesen, K. The cognitive bases of human tool use, Behavioral and brain sciences, vol.35, pp.203-262

A. Stoytchev, Robot tool behavior: A developmental approach to autonomous tool References use, 2007.

M. Tomasello and J. Call, Primate Cognition, 1997.

M. Tomasello, The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition, 1999.

T. Torigoe, Comparison of object manipulation among 74 species of non-human primates, Primates, vol.12, issue.2, pp.182-194, 1985.
DOI : 10.1007/BF02382017

I. C. Uzgiris and J. M. Hunt, Assessment in infancy : ordinal scales of psychological development, 1975.

K. Vaesen, Abstract, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, vol.22, issue.04, pp.203-218, 2012.
DOI : 10.1016/j.conb.2009.04.007

L. Van-leeuwen, A. Smitsman, and C. Van-leeuwen, Affordances, perceptual complexity, and the development of tool use., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.20, issue.1, pp.174-191, 1994.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.20.1.174

D. Van-roon, J. Van-der-kamp, and B. Steenbergen, Constraints in children's learning to use spoons, 2003.

J. Vauclair and K. A. Bard, Development of manipulations with objects in ape and human infants, Journal of Human Evolution, vol.12, issue.7, pp.631-645, 1983.
DOI : 10.1016/S0047-2484(83)80003-7

J. Vauclair, Tool use, hand cooperation and the development of object manipulation in human and non-human primates Cambridge, Motor development in early and later childhood: Longitudinal approaches, pp.205-216, 1993.

E. Visalberghi and L. Limongelli, Lack of comprehension of cause???ffect relations in tool-using capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)., Journal of Comparative Psychology, vol.108, issue.1, pp.15-22, 1994.
DOI : 10.1037/0735-7036.108.1.15

C. Von-hofsten, Action in Infancy: A Foundation for Cognitive Development, 2013.
DOI : 10.7551/mitpress/9780262018555.003.0010

C. Von-hofsten and L. Rönnqvist, Preparation for grasping an object: A developmental study., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.14, issue.4, pp.610-621, 1988.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.14.4.610

C. Von-hofsten, Action in development, Developmental Science, vol.3, issue.1, pp.54-60, 2007.
DOI : 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00564.x

C. Von-hofsten, An action perspective on motor development, Trends in Cognitive References Science, pp.266-272, 2004.
DOI : 10.1016/j.tics.2004.04.002

C. Von-hofsten, Developmental changes in the organization of prereaching movements., Developmental Psychology, vol.20, issue.3, pp.378-388, 1984.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.20.3.378

P. Willatts, Development of means-end behavior in young infants: Pulling a support to retrieve a distant object., Developmental Psychology, vol.35, issue.3, pp.651-667, 1999.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.35.3.651

P. Willats, The stage-IV infant's solution of problems requiring the use of supports, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.7, issue.2, pp.125-134, 1984.
DOI : 10.1016/S0163-6383(84)80053-3

P. H. Wolff, The serial organization of sucking in the young infant, Pediatrics, vol.42, issue.6, pp.943-956, 1968.

A. L. Woodward, J. A. Sommerville, S. Gerson, A. M. Henderson, and . Buresh, The emergence of intention attribution in infancy, The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, pp.187-222, 2009.

S. Zoia, L. Blason, G. Ottavio, M. Bulgheroni, E. Pezzetta et al., Evidence of early development of action planning in the human foetus: a kinematic study, Experimental Brain Research, vol.16, issue.2, pp.217-226, 2007.
DOI : 10.1007/s00221-006-0607-3

E. Bates, V. Carlsonluden, and I. Bretherton, Perceptual aspects of tool using in infancy, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.3, issue.2, pp.127-140, 1980.
DOI : 10.1016/S0163-6383(80)80017-8

E. Bates, D. Thal, L. Fenson, K. Whitesell, and L. Oakes, Integrating language and gesture in infancy., Developmental Psychology, vol.25, issue.6, pp.1004-1019, 1989.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.25.6.1004

A. L. Brown, Domain-Specific Principles Affect Learning and Transfer in Children, Cognitive Science, vol.14, issue.1, pp.107-133, 1990.

Z. Chen, R. P. Sanchez, and T. Campbell, From beyond to within their grasp: The rudiments of analogical problem solving in 10- and 13-month-olds., Developmental Psychology, vol.33, issue.5, pp.790-801, 1997.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.33.5.790

L. J. Claxton, M. E. Mccarty, and R. Keen, Self-directed action affects planning in tool-use tasks with toddlers, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.32, issue.2, pp.230-233, 2009.
DOI : 10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.12.004

K. Connolly and M. Dalgleish, The emergence of a tool-using skill in infancy., Developmental Psychology, vol.25, issue.6, pp.894-912, 1989.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.25.6.894

A. Diamond, Comparison of performance on a Piagetian object permanence task in human infants and rhesus monkeys : evidence for involvement of the prefrontal cortex, Neuroscience Abstracts, vol.9, p.641, 1983.

J. Fagard, L. Rat-fischer, and J. K. O-'regan, (soumis). The emergence of tool use: a longitudinal study in human infant

A. Gesell, Infancy and human growth, 1928.
DOI : 10.1037/14664-000

M. L. Greif and A. Needham, The Development of Human Tool Use Early in Life, pp.51-68, 2011.
DOI : 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199571154.003.0003

B. Koslowski and J. S. Bruner, Learning to Use a Lever, Child Development, vol.43, issue.3, pp.790-799, 1972.
DOI : 10.2307/1127631

A. M. Leslie and S. Keeble, Do six-month-old infants perceive causality? Cognition, pp.265-288, 1987.
DOI : 10.1016/s0010-0277(87)80006-9

URL : http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.357.3996

J. J. Lockman, A Perception-Action Perspective on Tool Use Development, Child Development, vol.71, issue.1, pp.137-144, 2000.
DOI : 10.1111/1467-8624.00127

M. E. Mccarty, R. K. Clifton, and R. R. Collard, Problem solving in infancy: The emergence of an action plan., Developmental Psychology, vol.35, issue.4, pp.1091-1101, 1999.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.35.4.1091

M. E. Mccarty, R. K. Clifton, and R. R. Collard, The Beginnings of Tool Use by Infants and Toddlers, Infancy, vol.1, issue.2, pp.233-256, 2001.
DOI : 10.1207/S15327078IN0202_8

O. 'regan, J. K. Rat-fischer, L. Fagard, and J. , Mechanisms leading to tool use: a longitudinal study, First Joint IEEE International Conference on Development and Learning and on Epigenetic Robotics ? Frankfurt Conference Proceedings, 2011.

J. Piaget, La naissance de l'intelligence chez l'enfant. Delachaux & Niestlé, 1936.

H. M. Richardson, The growth of adaptive behavior in infants: An experimental study at seven age levels, Genetic Psychology Monographs, XII, pp.195-357, 1932.

K. D. Sage and D. A. Baldwin, Disentangling the Social and the Pedagogical in Infants' Learning about Tool-Use, Social Development, vol.35, issue.4, pp.825-844, 2011.
DOI : 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011.00624.x

M. Schlesinger and J. Langer, Infants' developing expectations of possible and impossible tool-use events between ages 8 and 12 months, Developmental Science, vol.2, issue.2, pp.195-205, 1999.
DOI : 10.1111/1467-7687.00068

A. Seed and R. W. Byrne, Animal Tool-Use, Current Biology, vol.20, issue.23, 2010.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.042

J. A. Sommerville, E. A. Hildebrand, and C. C. Crane, Experience matters: The impact of doing versus watching on infants' subsequent perception of tool-use events., Developmental Psychology, vol.44, issue.5, pp.44-1249, 2008.
DOI : 10.1037/a0012296

I. C. Uzgiris, J. M. Hunt, A. Smitsman, and C. Van-leeuwen, Assessment in infancy : ordinal scales of psychological development Affordances, perceptual complexity, and the development of tool use, J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, vol.20, issue.1, pp.174-191, 1975.

C. P. Van-schaik, R. O. Deaner, and M. Y. Merrill, The conditions for tool use in primates: implications for the evolution of material culture, Journal of Human Evolution, vol.36, issue.6, pp.36-719, 1999.
DOI : 10.1006/jhev.1999.0304

P. Willatts, Development of means-end behavior in young infants: Pulling a support to retrieve a distant object., Developmental Psychology, vol.35, issue.3, pp.651-667, 1999.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.35.3.651

R. H. Wimmers, G. J. Savelsbergh, P. J. Beek, and B. Hopkins, Evidence for a phase transition in the early development of prehension, Developmental Psychobiology, vol.18, issue.3, pp.235-248, 1998.
DOI : 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199804)32:3<235::AID-DEV7>3.0.CO;2-P

T. Wynn, Piaget, stone tools and the evolution of human intelligence, World Archaeology, vol.17, issue.1, pp.32-43, 1985.
DOI : 10.1016/S0047-2484(81)80046-2

R. Baillargeon, R. Kotovsky, L. Needham, and A. , The acquisition of physical knowledge in infancy Causal cognition: A multidisciplinary debate, pp.79-116, 1995.

E. Bates, V. Carlsonluden, and I. Bretherton, Perceptual aspects of tool using in infancy, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.3, issue.2, pp.127-140, 1980.
DOI : 10.1016/S0163-6383(80)80017-8

A. L. Brown, Domain-Specific Principles Affect Learning and Transfer in Children, Cognitive Science, vol.14, issue.1, pp.107-133, 1990.

Z. Chen and R. S. Siegler, I. The Geat Dvide, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, vol.65, issue.2, pp.1-96, 2000.
DOI : 10.1111/1540-5834.00074

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01191235

K. Connolly and M. Dalgleish, The emergence of a tool-using skill in infancy., Developmental Psychology, vol.25, issue.6, pp.894-912, 1989.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.25.6.894

B. Elsner, Infants??? imitation of goal-directed actions: The role of movements and action effects, Acta Psychologica, vol.124, issue.1, pp.44-59, 2007.
DOI : 10.1016/j.actpsy.2006.09.006

R. Esseily, J. Nadel, and J. Fagard, Object retrieval through observational learning in 8- to 18-month-old infants, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.33, issue.4, pp.695-699, 2010.
DOI : 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.07.017

J. Fagard and J. J. Lockman, The effect of task constraints on infants??? (bi)manual strategy for grasping and exploring objects, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.28, issue.3, pp.305-315, 2005.
DOI : 10.1016/j.infbeh.2005.05.005

M. L. Greif and A. Needham, The Development of Human Tool Use Early in Life, McCormack, pp.51-68, 2011.
DOI : 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199571154.003.0003

R. Keen, The Development of Problem Solving in Young Children: A Critical Cognitive Skill, Annual Review of Psychology, vol.62, issue.1, 2011.
DOI : 10.1146/annurev.psych.031809.130730

W. Köhler, The mentality of apes New York: Harcourt, Brace. Appendix 3 ? The emergence of tool use: a longitudinal study in human infants, 1927.

B. Koslowski and J. S. Bruner, Learning to Use a Lever, Child Development, vol.43, issue.3, pp.790-799, 1972.
DOI : 10.2307/1127631

A. M. Leslie and S. Keeble, Do six-month-old infants perceive causality? Cognition, pp.265-288, 1987.

J. J. Lockman, A Perception-Action Perspective on Tool Use Development, Child Development, vol.71, issue.1, pp.137-144, 2000.
DOI : 10.1111/1467-8624.00127

M. E. Mccarty, R. K. Clifton, and R. R. Collard, Problem solving in infancy: The emergence of an action plan., Developmental Psychology, vol.35, issue.4, pp.1091-1101, 1999.
DOI : 10.1037/0012-1649.35.4.1091

P. Mounoud, The emergence of new skills : dialectic relations between knowledge systems The development of coordination in infancy, pp.13-46, 1993.

C. Nabeshima, Y. Kuniyoshi, and M. Lungarella, Adaptive body schema for robotic tool-use, Advanced Robotics, vol.20, issue.10, 2006.
DOI : 10.1163/156855306778522550

J. Piaget, The origins of intelligence in the child, 1952.

J. P. Piek, The role of variability in early motor development, Infant Behavior and Development, vol.25, issue.4, pp.452-465, 2002.
DOI : 10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00145-5

J. Provasi, C. D. Dubon, and H. Bloch, Do 9-and 12-month-olds learn means-ends relation by observing? Infant behavior and development, pp.195-213, 2001.

D. H. Rakinson and Y. Yermolayeva, How to Identify a Domain-General Learning Mechanism When You See One, Journal of Cognition and Development, vol.2, issue.2, pp.134-153, 2011.
DOI : 10.1073/pnas.0704450105

H. M. Richardson, The growth of adaptive behavior in infants: An experimental study at seven age levels, Genetic Psychology Monographs, XII, pp.195-357, 1932.

K. D. Sage and D. A. Baldwin, Disentangling the Social and the Pedagogical in Infants' Learning about Tool-Use, Social Development, vol.35, issue.4, pp.825-844, 2011.
DOI : 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011.00624.x

M. Schlesinger and J. Langer, Infants' developing expectations of possible and impossible tool-use events between ages 8 and 12 months, Developmental Science, vol.2, issue.2, pp.195-205, 1999.
DOI : 10.1111/1467-7687.00068

A. W. Smitsman and R. F. Cox, Perseveration in Tool Use: A Window for Understanding the Dynamics of the Action-Selection Process, Infancy, vol.11, issue.1, pp.249-269, 2008.
DOI : 10.1080/15250000802004379

E. Thelen, Motor development: A new synthesis., American Psychologist, vol.50, issue.2, pp.79-95, 1995.
DOI : 10.1037/0003-066X.50.2.79

E. Thelen, D. Corbetta, K. Kamm, J. P. Spencer, K. Schneider et al., The Transition to Reaching: Mapping Intention and Intrinsic Dynamics, Child Development, vol.64, issue.4, pp.1058-1098, 1993.
DOI : 10.2307/1131327

L. Van-leeuwen, A. Smitsman, and C. Van-leeuwen, Affordances, perceptual complexity, and the development of tool use., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.20, issue.1, pp.174-191, 1994.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.20.1.174

C. P. Van-schaik, R. O. Deaner, and M. Y. Merrill, The conditions for tool use in primates: implications for the evolution of material culture, Journal of Human Evolution, vol.36, issue.6, pp.36-719, 1999.
DOI : 10.1006/jhev.1999.0304

A. Voulomanos, Are U-Shaped Developmental Trajectories Illusory?, Journal of Cognition and Development, vol.12, issue.2, pp.154-158, 2011.
DOI : doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00789.x