B. L. Anderson, Transparency and occlusion The senses: A comprehensive reference, Vision II, vol.2, pp.239-244, 2008.

B. Anderson and K. Nakayama, Toward a general theory of stereopsis: Binocular matching, occluding contours, and fusion., Psychological Review, vol.101, issue.3, pp.414-445, 1994.
DOI : 10.1037/0033-295X.101.3.414

A. Assee and N. Qian, Solving da Vinci stereopsis with depth-edge-selective V2 cells, Vision Research, vol.47, issue.20, pp.2585-2602, 2007.
DOI : 10.1016/j.visres.2007.07.003

B. T. Backus, D. J. Fleet, A. J. Parker, and D. J. Heeger, Human cortical activity correlates with stereoscopic depth perception, Journal of Neurophysiology, vol.86, pp.2054-2068, 2001.

D. Brainard, The Psychophysics Toolbox, Spatial Vision, vol.10, issue.4, pp.433-436, 1997.
DOI : 10.1163/156856897X00357

M. Cook and B. Gillam, Depth of Monocular Elements in a Binocular Scene: The Conditions for da Vinci Stereopsis., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.30, issue.1, pp.92-103, 2004.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.30.1.92

D. Agostino, R. B. Belanger, A. Agostino, and R. B. , A suggestion for using powerful and informative tests of normality, The American Statistician, vol.44, issue.4, pp.31-321, 1990.

B. Efron and R. J. Tibshirani, An introduction to the bootstrap, 1994.
DOI : 10.1007/978-1-4899-4541-9

D. Geiger, B. Ladendorf, and A. Yuille, Occlusions and binocular stereo, International Journal of Computer Vision, vol.341, issue.2, pp.211-226, 1995.
DOI : 10.1007/BF01679683

B. Gillam, S. Blackburn, and M. Cook, Panum's Limiting Case: Double Fusion, Convergence Error, or ???Da Vinci Stereopsis???, Perception, vol.128, issue.3, pp.333-346, 1995.
DOI : 10.1068/p240333

B. Gillam and E. Borsting, The Role of Monocular Regions in Stereoscopic Displays, Perception, vol.221, issue.5, pp.603-608, 1988.
DOI : 10.1068/p170603

B. Gillam, M. Cook, and S. Blackburn, Monocular Discs in the Occlusion Zones of Binocular Surfaces Do Not Have Quantitative Depth???A Comparison with Panum's Limiting Case, Perception, vol.105, issue.8, pp.32-1009, 2003.
DOI : 10.1068/p3456

S. Grossberg and P. Howe, A laminar cortical model of stereopsis and three-dimensional surface perception, Vision Research, vol.43, issue.7, pp.801-829, 2003.
DOI : 10.1016/S0042-6989(03)00011-7

J. Häkkinen and G. Nyman, Depth Asymmetry in da Vinci Stereopsis, Vision Research, vol.36, issue.23, pp.3815-3819, 1996.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(96)00099-5

J. M. Harris and L. M. Wilcox, The role of monocularly visible regions in depth and surface perception, Vision Research, vol.49, issue.22, pp.2666-2685, 2009.
DOI : 10.1016/j.visres.2009.06.021

R. Hayashi, T. Maeda, S. Shimojo, and S. Tachi, An integrative model of binocular vision: a stereo model utilizing interocularly unpaired points produces both depth and binocular rivalry, Vision Research, vol.44, issue.20, pp.44-2367, 2004.
DOI : 10.1016/j.visres.2004.04.017

I. P. Howard and B. J. Rogers, Seeing in depth, 2002.
DOI : 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195367607.001.0001

D. H. Hubel and T. N. Wiesel, Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat's striate cortex, The Journal of Physiology, vol.148, issue.3, pp.574-591, 1959.
DOI : 10.1113/jphysiol.1959.sp006308

J. Jones and J. Malik, Computational framework for determining stereo correspondence from a set of linear spatial filters, Image and Vision Computing, vol.10, issue.10, 1992.
DOI : 10.1016/0262-8856(92)90015-U

R. Lawson and W. Gulick, Stereopsis and anomalous contour, Vision Research, vol.7, issue.3-4, pp.271-297, 1967.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(67)90091-0

P. Mamassian, M. S. Landy, and L. T. Maloney, Bayesian modelling of visual perception, Probabilistic models of the brain: Perception and neural function, pp.13-36, 2002.

F. Metelli, Stimulation and perception of transparency, Psychological Research, vol.10, issue.13, pp.185-202, 1985.
DOI : 10.1007/BF00309446

K. Nakayama and S. Shimojo, Da vinci stereopsis: Depth and subjective occluding contours from unpaired image points, Vision Research, vol.30, issue.11, pp.1811-1825, 1990.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(90)90161-D

H. Ono, K. Shimono, and K. Shibuta, Occlusion as a depth cue in the Wheatstone-Panum limiting case, Perception & Psychophysics, vol.128, issue.1, pp.3-13, 1992.
DOI : 10.3758/BF03205069

P. L. Panum, Untersuchungen uber das Sehen mit Zwei Augen, 1858.

A. J. Parker, Binocular depth perception and the cerebral cortex, Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol.87, issue.5, pp.379-391, 2007.
DOI : 10.1038/nrn2131

D. Pelli, The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies, Spatial Vision, vol.10, issue.4, pp.437-442, 1997.
DOI : 10.1163/156856897X00366

M. Pianta and B. Gillam, Paired and unpaired features can be equally effective in human depth perception, Vision Research, vol.43, issue.1, pp.1-6, 2003.
DOI : 10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00399-1

T. Porter and T. Duff, Compositing digital images, ACM SIGGRAPH Computer Graphics, vol.18, issue.3, pp.253-259, 1984.
DOI : 10.1145/964965.808606

T. P. Preston, S. Li, Z. Kourtzi, and A. E. Welchman, Multivoxel Pattern Selectivity for Perceptually Relevant Binocular Disparities in the Human Brain, Journal of Neuroscience, vol.28, issue.44, pp.11315-11327, 2008.
DOI : 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2728-08.2008

F. Qiu and R. Von-der-heydt, Neural representation of transparent overlay, Nature Neuroscience, vol.20, issue.3, pp.283-284, 2007.
DOI : 10.1068/p190497

A. B. Sekuler and S. E. Palmer, Perception of partly occluded objects: A microgenetic analysis., Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, vol.121, issue.1, pp.95-111, 1992.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-3445.121.1.95

M. Singh and B. Anderson, Toward a perceptual theory of transparency., Psychological Review, vol.109, issue.3, pp.492-519, 2002.
DOI : 10.1037/0033-295X.109.3.492

O. Watanabe and K. Fukushima, Stereo algorithm that extracts a depth cue from interocularly unpaired points Neural networks: The official journal of the International Neural Network Society, pp.4-5, 1999.

A. Treisman and G. Gelade, A feature-integration theory of attention, Cognitive Psychology, vol.12, issue.1, pp.97-136, 1980.
DOI : 10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5

M. Posner, Orienting of attention, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol.197, issue.1, pp.3-25, 1980.
DOI : 10.1080/00335558008248231

K. Nakayama and G. Silverman, Serial and parallel processing of visual feature conjunctions, Nature, vol.40, issue.6059, pp.264-265, 1986.
DOI : 10.1038/320264a0

J. Harris, S. Mckee, and S. Watamaniuk, Visual search for motion-indepth: stereomotion does not ''pop out'' from disparity noise, Nature Neuroscience, vol.1, issue.2, pp.165-168, 1998.
DOI : 10.1038/418

T. Andersen and P. Mamassian, Audiovisual integration of stimulus transients, Vision Research, vol.48, issue.25, pp.2537-2544, 2008.
DOI : 10.1016/j.visres.2008.08.018

T. Noesselt, S. Tyll, C. Boehler, E. Budinger, and H. Heinze, Sound-Induced Enhancement of Low-Intensity Vision: Multisensory Influences on Human Sensory-Specific Cortices and Thalamic Bodies Relate to Perceptual Enhancement of Visual Detection Sensitivity, Journal of Neuroscience, vol.30, issue.41, pp.13609-13623, 2010.
DOI : 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4524-09.2010

M. Lippert, N. Logothetis, and C. Kayser, Improvement of visual contrast detection by a simultaneous sound, Brain Research, vol.1173, pp.102-109, 2007.
DOI : 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.07.050

E. Van-der-burg, C. Olivers, A. Bronkhorst, and J. Theeuwes, Pip and pop: Nonspatial auditory signals improve spatial visual search., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.34, issue.5, pp.1053-1065, 2008.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.34.5.1053

E. Van-der-burg, J. Cass, C. Olivers, J. Theeuwes, D. Alais et al., Efficient Visual Search from Synchronized Auditory Signals Requires Transient Audiovisual Events Early multisensory interactions affect the competition among multiple visual objects, PLoS ONE NeuroImage, vol.5, issue.55, pp.1208-1218, 2010.

J. Harris, H. Nefs, and C. Grafton, Binocular vision and motion-in-depth, Spatial Vision, vol.21, issue.6, pp.531-547, 2008.
DOI : 10.1163/156856808786451462

D. Marr and T. Poggio, Cooperative computation of stereo disparity, Science, vol.194, issue.4262, pp.283-287, 1976.
DOI : 10.1126/science.968482

A. Assee and N. Qian, Solving da Vinci stereopsis with depth-edge-selective V2 cells, Vision Research, vol.47, issue.20, pp.2585-2602, 2007.
DOI : 10.1016/j.visres.2007.07.003

URL : http://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2007.07.003

H. Nienborg, H. Bridge, A. Parker, and B. Cumming, Neuronal Computation of Disparity in V1 Limits Temporal Resolution for Detecting Disparity Modulation, Journal of Neuroscience, vol.25, issue.44, pp.10207-10219, 2005.
DOI : 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2342-05.2005

I. Tsirlin, L. Wilcox, and R. Allison, The effect of crosstalk on the perceived depth from disparity and monocular occlusions. Broadcasting, IEEE Transactions on, pp.1-9, 2011.

K. Brooks and L. Stone, Spatial scale of stereomotion speed processing, Journal of Vision, vol.6, issue.11, pp.9-9, 2006.
DOI : 10.1167/6.11.9

D. Brainard, The Psychophysics Toolbox, Spatial Vision, vol.10, issue.4, pp.433-436, 1997.
DOI : 10.1163/156856897X00357

D. Pelli, The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies, Spatial Vision, vol.10, issue.4, pp.437-442, 1997.
DOI : 10.1163/156856897X00366

M. Zannoli, J. Cass, P. Mamassian, and D. Alais, Synchronized audio-visual transients drive efficient visual search for motion-in-depth, Journal of Vision, vol.1, issue.792, p.11, 2011.

R. Brainard and D. H. , The Psychophysics Toolbox, Spatial Vision, vol.10, issue.4, pp.433-436, 1997.
DOI : 10.1163/156856897X00357

K. R. Brooks, Interocular velocity difference contributes to stereomotion speed perception, Journal of Vision, vol.2, issue.3, 2002.
DOI : 10.1167/2.3.2

URL : http://doi.org/10.1167/2.3.2

K. R. Brooks and L. S. Stone, Stereomotion speed perception: Contributions from both changing disparity and interocular velocity difference over a range of relative disparities, Journal of Vision, vol.4, issue.12, 2004.
DOI : 10.1167/4.12.6

B. G. Cumming and A. J. Parker, Binocular mechanisms for detecting motion-in-depth, Vision Research, vol.34, issue.4, pp.483-495, 1994.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90162-7

T. B. Czuba, B. Rokers, A. C. Huk, and L. K. Cormack, Speed and Eccentricity Tuning Reveal a Central Role for the Velocity-Based Cue to 3D Visual Motion, Journal of Neurophysiology, vol.104, issue.5, pp.2886-2899, 2010.
DOI : 10.1152/jn.00585.2009

M. Edwards and J. A. Greenwood, The perception of motion transparency: A signal-tonoise limit, Vision Research, vol.45, issue.14, 2005.

J. A. Greenwood and M. Edwards, Pushing the limits of transparent-motion detection with binocular disparity, Vision Research, vol.46, issue.16, pp.2615-2624, 2006.
DOI : 10.1016/j.visres.2006.01.022

J. M. Harris, S. P. Mckee, and S. N. Watamaniuk, Visual search for motion-in-depth: Stereomotion does not ''pop out'' from disparity noise, Nature Neuroscience, vol.1, issue.2, pp.165-168, 1998.
DOI : 10.1038/418

J. M. Harris, H. T. Nefs, and C. E. Grafton, Binocular vision and motion-in-depth, Spatial Vision, vol.21, issue.6, pp.531-547, 2008.
DOI : 10.1163/156856808786451462

J. M. Harris and J. H. Sumnall, Detecting binocular 3D motion in static 3D noise: no effect of viewing distance, Spatial Vision, vol.14, issue.1, pp.11-19, 2000.
DOI : 10.1163/156856801741332

J. M. Harris and S. N. Watamaniuk, Speed discrimination of motion-in-depth using binocular cues, Vision Research, vol.35, issue.7, pp.885-896, 1995.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(94)00194-Q

P. B. Hibbard and M. F. Bradshaw, Does binocular disparity facilitate the detection of transparent motion? Perception, pp.183-191, 1999.

Y. Huang and R. P. Rao, Predictive coding, Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, vol.93, issue.60, pp.580-593, 2011.
DOI : 10.1002/wcs.142

B. Julesz, Binocular Depth Perception of Computer-Generated Patterns, Bell System Technical Journal, vol.39, issue.5, pp.1125-1162, 1960.
DOI : 10.1002/j.1538-7305.1960.tb03954.x

J. Lewald and R. Guski, Cross-modal perceptual integration of spatially and temporally disparate auditory and visual stimuli, Cognitive Brain Research, vol.16, issue.3, pp.468-478, 2003.
DOI : 10.1016/S0926-6410(03)00074-0

L. T. Likova and C. W. Tyler, Stereomotion processing in the human occipital cortex, NeuroImage, vol.38, issue.2, pp.293-305, 2007.
DOI : 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.06.039

P. Mamassian and J. M. Wallace, Sustained directional biases in motion transparency Journal o f V i s i o n, pp.1-1, 2010.

D. Marr and T. Poggio, Cooperative computation of stereo disparity, Science, vol.194, issue.4262, pp.283-287, 1976.
DOI : 10.1126/science.968482

J. H. Maunsell and D. C. Van-essen, Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. II. Binocular interactions and sensitivity to binocular disparity, Journal of Neurophysiology, vol.49, issue.5, pp.1148-1167, 1983.

K. Moutoussis and S. Zeki, A direct demonstration of perceptual asynchrony in vision, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, vol.264, issue.1380, pp.393-399, 1380.
DOI : 10.1098/rspb.1997.0056

K. Moutoussis and S. Zeki, Functional segregation and temporal hierarchy of the visual perceptive systems, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, vol.264, issue.1387, pp.1407-1414, 1387.
DOI : 10.1098/rspb.1997.0196

H. T. Nefs and J. M. Harris, What visual information is used for stereoscopic depth displacement discrimination? Perception, pp.727-744, 2010.

A. M. Norcia and C. W. Tyler, Temporal frequency limits for stereoscopic apparent motion processes, Vision Research, vol.24, issue.5, pp.395-401, 1984.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(84)90037-3

D. G. Pelli, The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies, Spatial Vision, vol.10, issue.4, pp.437-442, 1997.
DOI : 10.1163/156856897X00366

R. P. Rao and D. H. Ballard, Predictive coding in the visual cortex: A functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects, Nature Neuroscience, vol.2, issue.1, pp.79-87, 1999.
DOI : 10.1038/4580

C. Rashbass and G. Westheimer, Disjunctive eye movements, The Journal of Physiology, vol.159, issue.2, pp.339-360, 1961.
DOI : 10.1113/jphysiol.1961.sp006812

URL : http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1359509

D. Regan and K. Beverley, Some dynamic features of depth perception, Vision Research, vol.13, issue.12, pp.2369-2379, 1973.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(73)90236-8

N. W. Roach, P. V. Mcgraw, and A. Johnston, Visual Motion Induces a Forward Prediction of Spatial Pattern, Current Biology, vol.21, issue.9, pp.740-745, 2011.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cub.2011.03.031

B. Rokers, L. K. Cormack, and A. C. Huk, Disparity-and velocity-based signals for threedimensional motion perception in human MTþ, Nature Neuroscience, issue.8, pp.12-1050, 2009.

B. Rokers, T. B. Czuba, L. K. Cormack, and A. C. Huk, Motion processing with two eyes in three dimensions, Journal of Vision, vol.11, issue.2, pp.1-19, 2011.
DOI : 10.1167/11.2.10

J. M. Samonds, B. R. Potetz, and T. S. Lee, Cooperative and Competitive Interactions Facilitate Stereo Computations in Macaque Primary Visual Cortex, Journal of Neuroscience, vol.29, issue.50, pp.29-15780, 2009.
DOI : 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2305-09.2009

S. Shioiri, T. Nakajima, D. Kakehi, and H. Yaguchi, Differences in temporal frequency tuning between the two binocular mechanisms for seeing motion in depth, Journal of the Optical Society of America A, vol.25, issue.7, pp.25-1574, 2008.
DOI : 10.1364/JOSAA.25.001574

R. J. Snowden and M. C. Rossiter, Stereoscopic Depth Cues Can Segment Motion Information, Perception, vol.33, issue.2, pp.193-201, 1999.
DOI : 10.1068/p2735

C. W. Tyler, Stereoscopic Depth Movement: Two Eyes Less Sensitive than One, Science, vol.174, issue.4012, pp.958-961, 1971.
DOI : 10.1126/science.174.4012.958

W. R. Uttal, N. S. Davis, and C. Welke, Stereoscopic perception with brief exposures, Perception & Psychophysics, vol.15, issue.5, pp.599-604, 1994.
DOI : 10.3758/BF03206955

S. Zeki and A. Bartels, The asynchrony of consciousness, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, vol.265, issue.1405, pp.1583-1585, 1405.
DOI : 10.1098/rspb.1998.0475

D. Alais and D. Burr, The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration, Current Biology, vol.14, issue.3, pp.257-262, 2004.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cub.2004.01.029

D. Alais, J. Cass, R. P. O-'shea, and R. Blake, Visual Sensitivity Underlying Changes in Visual Consciousness, Current Biology, vol.20, issue.15, pp.20-1362, 2010.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cub.2010.06.015

R. S. Allison, I. P. Howard, and A. Howard, Motion in depth can be elicited by dichoptically uncorrelated textures, Perception, issue.2, p.46, 1998.

J. Almeida, B. Z. Mahon, K. Nakayama, and A. Caramazza, Unconscious processing dissociates along categorical lines, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol.105, issue.39, pp.105-15214, 2008.
DOI : 10.1073/pnas.0805867105

URL : http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2567517

A. Amedi, N. Raz, P. Pianka, R. Malach, and E. Zohary, Early 'visual' cortex activation correlates with superior verbal memory performance in the blind, Nature Neuroscience, vol.6, issue.7, pp.758-766, 2003.
DOI : 10.1038/nn1072

T. S. Andersen and P. Mamassian, Audiovisual integration of stimulus transients, Vision Research, vol.48, issue.25, pp.2537-2544, 2008.
DOI : 10.1016/j.visres.2008.08.018

B. L. Anderson, The role of partial occlusion in stereopsis, Nature, vol.367, issue.6461, pp.365-368, 1994.
DOI : 10.1038/367365a0

B. L. Anderson and K. Nakayama, Toward a general theory of stereopsis: Binocular matching, occluding contours, and fusion., Psychological Review, vol.101, issue.3, pp.414-445, 1994.
DOI : 10.1037/0033-295X.101.3.414

P. A. Anderson and J. A. Movshon, Binocular combination of contrast signals, Vision Research, vol.29, issue.9, pp.1115-1132, 1989.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(89)90060-6

T. J. Andrews, A. Glennerster, and A. J. Parker, Stereoacuity thresholds in the presence of a reference surface, Vision Research, vol.41, issue.23, pp.3051-3061, 2001.
DOI : 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00192-4

A. Assee and N. Qian, Solving da Vinci stereopsis with depth-edge-selective V2 cells, Vision Research, vol.47, issue.20, pp.2585-2602, 2007.
DOI : 10.1016/j.visres.2007.07.003

B. T. Backus and M. S. Banks, Estimator Reliability and Distance Scaling in Stereoscopic Slant Perception, Perception, vol.76, issue.2, pp.217-242, 1999.
DOI : 10.1002/0471725250

B. T. Backus, M. S. Banks, R. Van-ee, and J. A. Crowell, Horizontal and vertical disparity, eye position, and stereoscopic slant perception, Vision Research, vol.39, issue.6, pp.1143-1170, 1999.
DOI : 10.1016/S0042-6989(98)00139-4

URL : http://doi.org/10.1016/s0042-6989(98)00139-4

B. T. Backus, D. J. Fleet, A. J. Parker, and D. J. Heeger, Human cortical activity correlates with stereoscopic depth perception, Journal of Neurophysiology, vol.86, issue.4, pp.2054-2068, 2001.

H. Ban, T. J. Preston, A. Meeson, and A. E. Welchman, The integration of motion and disparity cues to depth in dorsal visual cortex, Nature Neuroscience, vol.86, issue.4, pp.636-643, 2012.
DOI : 10.1038/nn1444

M. S. Banks, S. Gepshtein, and M. S. Landy, Why Is Spatial Stereoresolution So Low?, Journal of Neuroscience, vol.24, issue.9, pp.2077-2089, 2004.
DOI : 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3852-02.2004

URL : http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.5.5683

H. B. Barlow, C. Blakemore, and J. D. Pettigrew, The neural mechanism of binocular depth discrimination, The Journal of Physiology, vol.193, issue.2, pp.327-342, 1967.
DOI : 10.1113/jphysiol.1967.sp008360

M. S. Beauchamp, K. E. Lee, B. D. Argall, and A. Martin, Integration of Auditory and Visual Information about Objects in Superior Temporal Sulcus, Neuron, vol.41, issue.5, pp.41-809, 2004.
DOI : 10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00070-4

M. S. Beauchamp, N. E. Yasar, R. E. Frye, and T. Ro, Touch, sound and ! CG! vision in human superior temporal sulcus, NeuroImage, issue.3, pp.41-1011, 2008.

K. I. Beverley and D. Regan, Evidence for the existence of neural mechanisms selectively sensitive to the direction of movement in space, The Journal of Physiology, vol.235, issue.1, pp.17-29, 1973.
DOI : 10.1113/jphysiol.1973.sp010376

R. Blake, A neural theory of binocular rivalry., Psychological Review, vol.96, issue.1, pp.145-167, 1989.
DOI : 10.1037/0033-295X.96.1.145

R. Blake and R. Fox, The psychophysical inquiry into binocular summation. Attention, perception & psychophysics, pp.161-185, 1973.

R. Blake, K. V. Sobel, and L. A. Gilroy, Visual Motion Retards Alternations between Conflicting Perceptual Interpretations, Neuron, vol.39, issue.5, pp.869-878, 2003.
DOI : 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00495-1

URL : http://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00495-1

D. H. Brainard, The Psychophysics Toolbox, Spatial Vision, vol.10, issue.4, pp.433-436, 1997.
DOI : 10.1163/156856897X00357

C. E. Bredfeldt and B. G. Cumming, A Simple Account of Cyclopean Edge Responses in Macaque V2, Journal of Neuroscience, vol.26, issue.29, pp.7581-7596, 2006.
DOI : 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5308-05.2006

K. Brooks, Stereomotion Speed Perception is Contrast Dependent, Perception, vol.24, issue.4, pp.725-731, 2001.
DOI : 10.1068/p3143

K. R. Brooks, Interocular velocity difference contributes to stereomotion speed perception, Journal of Vision, vol.2, issue.3, pp.218-231, 2002.
DOI : 10.1167/2.3.2

K. R. Brooks, Monocular motion adaptation affects the perceived trajectory of stereomotion., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.28, issue.6, pp.1470-1482, 2002.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.28.6.1470

K. R. Brooks and L. S. Stone, Stereomotion speed perception: Contributions from both changing disparity and interocular velocity difference over a range of relative disparities, Journal of Vision, vol.4, issue.12, pp.6-6, 2004.
DOI : 10.1167/4.12.6

K. R. Brooks and L. S. Stone, Spatial scale of stereomotion speed processing, Journal of Vision, vol.6, issue.11, pp.9-9, 2006.
DOI : 10.1167/6.11.9

K. Brooks and B. Gillam, Quantitative perceived depth from sequential monocular decamouflage, Vision Research, vol.46, issue.5, pp.605-613, 2006.
DOI : 10.1016/j.visres.2005.06.015

D. Burr and D. Alais, Combining visual and auditory information Presented at the Visual Perception, Pt 2: Fundamentals of Awareness: Multi-Sensory Integration and High-Order Perception, pp.243-258, 2006.

P. Burt and B. Julesz, A disparity gradient limit for binocular fusion, Science, vol.208, issue.4444, pp.615-617, 1980.
DOI : 10.1126/science.7367885

G. A. Calvert, E. T. Bullmore, M. J. Brammer, R. Campbell, S. C. Williams et al., Activation of Auditory Cortex During Silent Lipreading, Science, vol.276, issue.5312, pp.276-593, 1997.
DOI : 10.1126/science.276.5312.593

G. A. Calvert, R. Campbell, and M. J. Brammer, Evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging of crossmodal binding in the human heteromodal cortex, Current Biology, vol.10, issue.11, pp.10-649, 2000.
DOI : 10.1016/S0960-9822(00)00513-3

G. A. Calvert, B. E. Stein, and C. Spence, The handbook of multisensory processes, 2004.

F. W. Campbell and D. G. Green, Monocular versus Binocular Visual Acuity, Nature, vol.141, issue.5006, pp.191-192, 1965.
DOI : 10.1085/jgp.21.2.165

C. Cappe and P. Barone, Heteromodal connections supporting multisensory integration at low levels of cortical processing in the monkey, European Journal of Neuroscience, vol.6, issue.11, pp.2886-2902, 2005.
DOI : 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04462.x

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00111035

T. A. Carlson and S. He, Visible binocular beats from invisible monocular stimuli during binocular rivalry, Current Biology, vol.10, issue.17, pp.1055-1058, 2000.
DOI : 10.1016/S0960-9822(00)00672-2

URL : http://doi.org/10.1016/s0960-9822(00)00672-2

S. C. Chong, D. Tadin, and R. Blake, Endogenous attention prolongs dominance durations in binocular rivalry, Journal of Vision, vol.5, issue.11, pp.1004-1012, 2005.
DOI : 10.1167/5.11.6

A. Chopin and P. Mamassian, Predictive Properties of Visual Adaptation, Current Biology, vol.22, issue.7, pp.622-626, 2012.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cub.2012.02.021

M. Cook and B. Gillam, Depth of Monocular Elements in a Binocular Scene: The Conditions for da Vinci Stereopsis., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.30, issue.1, pp.92-103, 2004.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.30.1.92

L. K. Cormack, S. B. Stevenson, and C. M. Schor, Interocular correlation, luminance contrast and cyclopean processing, Vision Research, vol.31, issue.12, pp.31-2195, 1991.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(91)90172-2

B. Cumming, The Relationship between Stereoacuity and Stereomotion Thresholds, Perception, vol.142, issue.1, pp.105-114, 1995.
DOI : 10.1068/p240105

B. Cumming, Binocular Neurons in V1 of Awake Monkeys Are Selective for Absolute, Not Relative, Disparity. The Journal of neuroscience, 1999.

B. G. Cumming and A. J. Parker, Binocular mechanisms for detecting motion-in-depth, Vision Research, vol.34, issue.4, pp.483-495, 1994.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90162-7

B. G. Cumming and A. J. Parker, Responses of primary visual cortical neurons to binocular disparity without depth perception, Nature, issue.6648, pp.389-280, 1997.

M. Cynader and D. Regan, Neurones in cat parastriate cortex sensitive to the direction of motion in three-dimensional space, The Journal of Physiology, vol.274, issue.1, pp.549-569, 1978.
DOI : 10.1113/jphysiol.1978.sp012166

M. Cynader and D. Regan, Neurons in cat visual cortex tuned to the direction of motion in depth: Effect of positional disparity, Vision Research, vol.22, issue.8, pp.967-982, 1982.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(82)90033-5

T. B. Czuba, B. Rokers, K. Guillet, A. C. Huk, and L. K. Cormack, Threedimensional motion aftereffects reveal distinct direction-selective mechanisms for binocular processing of motion through depth, Journal of Vision, issue.10, pp.11-18, 2011.

T. B. Czuba, B. Rokers, A. C. Huk, and L. K. Cormack, Speed and Eccentricity Tuning Reveal a Central Role for the Velocity-Based Cue to 3D Visual Motion, Journal of Neurophysiology, vol.104, issue.5, pp.2886-2899, 2010.
DOI : 10.1152/jn.00585.2009

. Der-burg, E. Van, J. Cass, C. N. Olivers, J. Theeuwes et al., Efficient Visual Search from Synchronized Auditory Signals Requires Transient Audiovisual Events, PLoS ONE, vol.11, issue.5, p.10664, 2010.
DOI : 10.1371/journal.pone.0010664.s008

E. Diaz-caneja, Sur l'alternance binoculaire, Annales D'Oculistique, vol.165, pp.721-731, 1928.

J. Ding and D. M. Levi, Recovery of stereopsis through perceptual learning in human adults with abnormal binocular vision, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol.108, issue.37, pp.733-774, 2011.
DOI : 10.1073/pnas.1105183108

J. Ding and G. Sperling, A gain-control theory of binocular combination, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol.103, issue.4, pp.1141-1146, 2006.
DOI : 10.1073/pnas.0509629103

J. Driver and T. Noesselt, Multisensory Interplay Reveals Crossmodal Influences on " Sensory-Specific, Brain Regions, Neural Responses, and Judgments. Neuron, vol.57, issue.1, pp.11-23, 2008.

J. Durand, S. Celebrini, and Y. Trotter, Neural Bases of Stereopsis across Visual Field of the Alert Macaque Monkey, Cerebral Cortex, vol.17, issue.6, pp.17-1260, 1991.
DOI : 10.1093/cercor/bhl050

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00159263

J. Durand, S. Zhu, S. Celebrini, and Y. Trotter, Neurons in Parafoveal Areas V1 and V2 Encode Vertical and Horizontal Disparities, Journal of Neurophysiology, vol.88, issue.5, pp.2874-2879, 2002.
DOI : 10.1152/jn.00291.2002

C. J. Erkelens and H. Collewijn, Motion perception during dichoptic viewing of moving random-dot stereograms, Vision Research, vol.25, issue.4, pp.583-588, 1985.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(85)90164-6

M. O. Ernst and M. S. Banks, Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion, Nature, vol.415, issue.6870, pp.415-429, 2002.
DOI : 10.1038/415429a

M. O. Ernst and H. H. Bülthoff, Merging the senses into a robust percept, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol.8, issue.4, pp.162-169, 2004.
DOI : 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.002

A. Falchier, S. Clavagnier, P. Barone, and H. Kennedy, Anatomical evidence of multimodal integration in primate striate cortex, Journal of Neuroscience, vol.22, issue.13, pp.5749-5759, 2002.

J. M. Fernandez and B. Farell, Seeing motion in depth using inter-ocular velocity differences, Vision Research, vol.45, issue.21, pp.2786-2798, 2005.
DOI : 10.1016/j.visres.2005.05.021

H. R. Filippini and M. S. Banks, Limits of stereopsis explained by local crosscorrelation, Journal of Vision, vol.9, issue.8, pp.1-18, 2009.

E. M. Finney, I. Fine, and K. R. Dobkins, Visual stimuli activate auditory cortex in the deaf, Nature Neuroscience, vol.4, issue.12, pp.1171-1173, 2001.
DOI : 10.1038/nn763

J. J. Foxe, G. R. Wylie, A. Martinez, C. E. Schroeder, D. C. Javitt et al., Auditory-somatosensory multisensory processing in auditory association cortex: an fMRI study, Journal of Neurophysiology, vol.88, issue.1, pp.540-543, 2002.

A. W. Freeman, Multistage Model for Binocular Rivalry, Journal of Neurophysiology, vol.94, issue.6, pp.4412-4420, 2005.
DOI : 10.1152/jn.00557.2005

K. G. Fu, T. A. Johnston, A. S. Shah, L. Arnold, J. Smiley et al., Auditory cortical neurons respond to somatosensory stimulation, Journal of Neuroscience, vol.23, issue.20, pp.7510-7515, 2003.

D. Geiger and B. Ladendorf, Occlusions and binocular stereo, International Journal of Computer Vision, 1995.
DOI : 10.1007/3-540-55426-2_48

B. Gillam and B. Lawergren, The induced effect, vertical disparity, and stereoscopic theory, Perception & Psychophysics, vol.17, issue.1, pp.121-130, 1983.
DOI : 10.3758/BF03211336

B. Gillam, S. Blackburn, and M. Cook, Panum's Limiting Case: Double Fusion, Convergence Error, or ???Da Vinci Stereopsis???, Perception, vol.128, issue.3, pp.333-346, 1995.
DOI : 10.1068/p240333

B. Gillam, S. Blackburn, and K. Nakayama, Stereopsis based on monocular gaps: Metrical encoding of depth and slant without matching contours1This work was first reported at ARVO (Gillam & Nakayama, 1995). Supported by an ARC grant to B. Gillam and an AFOSR and HFSP grant to K. Nakayama.1, Vision Research, vol.39, issue.3, pp.493-502, 1999.
DOI : 10.1016/S0042-6989(98)00131-X

B. Gillam and E. Borsting, The Role of Monocular Regions in Stereoscopic Displays, Perception, vol.221, issue.5, pp.603-608, 1988.
DOI : 10.1068/p170603

B. Gillam, M. Cook, and S. Blackburn, Monocular Discs in the Occlusion Zones of Binocular Surfaces Do Not Have Quantitative Depth???A Comparison with Panum's Limiting Case, Perception, vol.105, issue.8, pp.32-1009, 2003.
DOI : 10.1068/p3456

F. Gonzalez, M. S. Justo, M. A. Bermudez, and R. Perez, Sensitivity to horizontal and vertical disparity and orientation preference in areas V1 and V2 of the monkey, NeuroReport, vol.14, issue.6, pp.14-829, 2003.
DOI : 10.1097/00001756-200305060-00010

M. A. Goodale and A. D. Milner, Separate visual pathways for perception and action, Trends in Neurosciences, vol.15, issue.1, pp.20-25, 1992.
DOI : 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8

R. Goutcher and P. Mamassian, Selective biasing of stereo correspondence in an ambiguous stereogram, Vision Research, vol.45, issue.4, pp.469-483, 2005.
DOI : 10.1016/j.visres.2004.08.025

M. S. Goyal, P. J. Hansen, and C. B. Blakemore, Tactile perception recruits functionally related visual areas in the late-blind, NeuroReport, vol.17, issue.13, pp.17-1381, 2006.
DOI : 10.1097/01.wnr.0000227990.23046.fe

S. Grossberg and P. D. Howe, A laminar cortical model of stereopsis and three-dimensional surface perception, Vision Research, vol.43, issue.7, pp.801-829, 2003.
DOI : 10.1016/S0042-6989(03)00011-7

P. M. Grove, B. Sachtler, W. L. Gillam, and B. J. , Amodal completion with background determines depth from monocular gap stereopsis, Vision Research, vol.46, issue.22, pp.3771-3774, 2006.
DOI : 10.1016/j.visres.2006.06.020

P. M. Grove, B. Gillam, and H. Ono, Content and context of monocular regions determine perceived depth in random dot, unpaired background and phantom stereograms, Vision Research, vol.42, issue.15, pp.42-1859, 2002.
DOI : 10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00083-4

J. M. Harris and J. H. Sumnall, Detecting binocular 3D motion in static 3D noise: no effect of viewing distance, Spatial Vision, vol.14, issue.1, pp.11-19, 2000.
DOI : 10.1163/156856801741332

J. M. Harris and S. N. Watamaniuk, Speed discrimination of motion-in-depth using binocular cues, Vision Research, vol.35, issue.7, pp.885-896, 1995.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(94)00194-Q

J. M. Harris, S. P. Mckee, and H. S. Smallman, Fine-scale processing in human binocular stereopsis, Journal of the Optical Society of America A, vol.14, issue.8, pp.14-1673, 1997.
DOI : 10.1364/JOSAA.14.001673

J. M. Harris, S. P. Mckee, and S. N. Watamaniuk, Visual search for motionin-depth: stereomotion does not " pop out " from disparity noise, Nature Neuroscience, vol.1, issue.2, pp.165-168, 1998.
DOI : 10.1038/418

J. M. Harris, H. T. Nefs, and C. E. Grafton, Binocular vision and motion-in-depth, Spatial Vision, vol.21, issue.6, pp.531-547, 2008.
DOI : 10.1163/156856808786451462

R. Hayashi, T. Maeda, S. Shimojo, and S. Tachi, An integrative model of binocular vision: a stereo model utilizing interocularly unpaired points produces both depth and binocular rivalry, Vision Research, vol.44, issue.20, pp.44-2367, 2004.
DOI : 10.1016/j.visres.2004.04.017

J. Häkkinen and G. Nyman, Depth Asymmetry in da Vinci Stereopsis, Vision Research, vol.36, issue.23, pp.3815-3819, 1996.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(96)00099-5

Z. J. He and K. Nakayama, Visual attention to surfaces in three-dimensional space., Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol.92, issue.24, pp.92-11155, 1995.
DOI : 10.1073/pnas.92.24.11155

G. Hein, O. Doehrmann, N. G. Müller, J. Kaiser, L. Muckli et al., Object Familiarity and Semantic Congruency Modulate Responses in Cortical Audiovisual Integration Areas, Journal of Neuroscience, vol.27, issue.30, pp.27-7881, 2007.
DOI : 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1740-07.2007

E. Hering, Beiträge zur Physiologie, Leipzig: Engelmann, 1861.

I. P. Howard, Seeing in depth (Vol. I), 2002.

I. P. Howard and B. J. Rogers, Seeing in depth, 2002.
DOI : 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195367607.001.0001

D. H. Hubel and T. N. Wiesel, Receptive fields of single neurones in the cat's striate cortex, The Journal of Physiology, vol.148, issue.3, pp.574-591, 1959.
DOI : 10.1113/jphysiol.1959.sp006308

D. H. Hubel and T. N. Wiesel, Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex, The Journal of Physiology, vol.160, issue.1, pp.106-154, 1962.
DOI : 10.1113/jphysiol.1962.sp006837

D. Humphriss, The Psychological Septum. An Investigation into Its Function, Optometry and Vision Science, vol.59, issue.8, pp.639-680, 1982.
DOI : 10.1097/00006324-198208000-00004

P. Janssen, R. Vogels, and G. A. Orban, Three-Dimensional Shape Coding in Inferior Temporal Cortex, Neuron, vol.27, issue.2, pp.385-397, 2000.
DOI : 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)00045-3

P. Janssen, R. Vogels, Y. Liu, and G. A. Orban, At Least at the Level of Inferior Temporal Cortex, the Stereo Correspondence Problem Is Solved, Neuron, vol.37, issue.4, pp.693-701, 2003.
DOI : 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00023-0

Y. Jiang and S. He, Cortical Responses to Invisible Faces: Dissociating Subsystems for Facial-Information Processing, Current Biology, vol.16, issue.20, pp.2023-2029, 2006.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cub.2006.08.084

Y. Jiang, P. Costello, and S. He, Processing of Invisible Stimuli: Advantage of Upright Faces and Recognizable Words in Overcoming Interocular Suppression, Psychological Science, vol.10, issue.4, 2007.
DOI : 10.1037//0096-1523.9.5.807

D. G. Jones and J. Malik, Computational framework for determining stereo correspondence from a set of linear spatial filters, Image and Vision Computing, vol.10, issue.10, pp.699-708, 1992.
DOI : 10.1016/0262-8856(92)90015-U

R. K. Jones and D. N. Lee, Why two eyes are better than one: The two views of binocular vision., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.7, issue.1, pp.30-40, 1981.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.7.1.30

B. Julesz, Binocular Depth Perception of Computer-Generated Patterns, Bell System Technical Journal, vol.39, issue.5, pp.1125-1162, 1960.
DOI : 10.1002/j.1538-7305.1960.tb03954.x

B. Julesz, Binocular Depth Perception without Familiarity Cues: Random-dot stereo images with controlled spatial and temporal properties clarify problems in stereopsis, Science, vol.145, issue.3630, pp.356-362, 1964.
DOI : 10.1126/science.145.3630.356

B. Julesz, Binocular depth perception without familiarity cues, Science, issue.3630, pp.145-356, 1964.

B. Julesz and C. W. Tyler, Neurontropy, an entropy-like measure of neural correlation, in binocular fusion and rivalry, Biological Cybernetics, vol.16, issue.1, pp.25-32, 1976.
DOI : 10.1007/BF00344148

T. Kanade and M. Okutomi, A stereo matching algorithm with an adaptive window: Theory and experiment. Robotics and Automation, IEEE International Conference on, pp.1088-1095, 1991.

L. Kaufman, Suppression and Fusion in Viewing Complex Stereograms, The American Journal of Psychology, vol.77, issue.2, pp.193-205, 1964.
DOI : 10.2307/1420126

R. S. Kim, A. R. Seitz, and L. Shams, Benefits of Stimulus Congruency for Multisensory Facilitation of Visual Learning, PLoS ONE, vol.50, issue.2, p.1532, 2008.
DOI : 10.1371/journal.pone.0001532.g003

R. Kim, M. A. Peters, and L. Shams, 0 + 1 > 1, Psychological Science, vol.65, issue.1, pp.6-12, 2012.
DOI : 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.016

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/in2p3-00018511

H. Kitaoji and K. Toyama, Preservation of position and motion stereopsis in strabismic subjects, Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, vol.28, issue.8, pp.1260-1267, 1987.

I. Kovács, T. V. Papathomas, M. Yang, and Á. Fehér, When the brain changes its mind: Interocular grouping during binocular rivalry, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol.93, issue.26, pp.93-15508, 1996.
DOI : 10.1073/pnas.93.26.15508

T. Kujala, M. Huotilainen, J. Sinkkonen, A. I. Ahonen, K. Alho et al., Visual cortex activation in blind humans during sound discrimination, Neuroscience Letters, vol.183, issue.1-2, pp.143-146, 1995.
DOI : 10.1016/0304-3940(94)11135-6

R. B. Lawson and W. L. Gulick, Stereopsis and anomalous contour, Vision Research, vol.7, issue.3-4, pp.271-297, 1967.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(67)90091-0

G. E. Legge, Binocular contrast summation???I. Detection and discrimination, Vision Research, vol.24, issue.4, pp.373-383, 1984.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(84)90063-4

G. E. Legge, Binocular contrast summation???II. Quadratic summation, Vision Research, vol.24, issue.4, pp.385-394, 1984.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(84)90064-6

S. R. Lehky, An Astable Multivibrator Model of Binocular Rivalry, Perception, vol.93, issue.2, pp.215-228, 1988.
DOI : 10.1068/p170215

W. Levelt, THE ALTERNATION PROCESS IN BINOCULAR RIVALRY, British Journal of Psychology, vol.57, issue.3-4, pp.225-238, 1966.
DOI : 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1966.tb01023.x

W. J. Levelt, BINOCULAR BRIGHTNESS AVERAGING AND CONTOUR INFORMATION, British Journal of Psychology, vol.56, issue.1, pp.1-13, 1965.
DOI : 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1965.tb00939.x

L. T. Likova and C. W. Tyler, Stereomotion processing in the human occipital cortex, NeuroImage, vol.38, issue.2, pp.293-305, 2007.
DOI : 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.06.039

M. Maeda, M. Sato, T. Ohmura, Y. Miyazaki, A. Wang et al., Binocular depth-from-motion in infantile and late-onset esotropia patients with poor stereopsis, Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, issue.12, pp.40-3031, 1999.

P. Mamassian, Depth, but not surface orientation, from binocular disparities, Journal of Vision, vol.8, issue.6, 2008.
DOI : 10.1167/8.6.89

&. Marr and . Poggio, A Theory of Human Stereo Vision, pp.1-89, 1979.

D. Marr and T. Poggio, Cooperative computation of stereo disparity, Science, vol.194, issue.4262, pp.283-287, 1976.
DOI : 10.1126/science.968482

G. S. Masson, C. Busettini, and F. A. Miles, Vergence eye movements in response to binocular disparity without depth perception, Nature, issue.6648, pp.389-283, 1997.

J. H. Maunsell and D. C. Van-essen, Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. II. Binocular interactions and sensitivity to binocular disparity, Journal of Neurophysiology, vol.49, issue.5, pp.1148-1167, 1983.

J. Mayhew and H. Longuet-higgins, A computational model of binocular depth perception, Nature, vol.208, issue.5865, pp.376-378, 1982.
DOI : 10.1038/297376a0

T. S. Meese, M. A. Georgeson, and D. H. Baker, Binocular contrast vision at and above threshold, Journal of Vision, vol.6, issue.11, pp.1224-1243, 2006.
DOI : 10.1167/6.11.7

M. A. Meredith and B. E. Stein, The visuotopic component of the multisensory map in the deep laminae of the cat superior colliculus, The Journal of neuroscience, vol.10, issue.11, pp.3727-3742, 1990.

M. A. Meredith and B. E. Stein, The Merging of the Senses, 2003.

T. J. Mueller and R. Blake, A fresh look at the temporal dynamics of binocular rivalry, Biological Cybernetics, vol.128, issue.3, pp.61-223, 1989.
DOI : 10.1007/BF00198769

K. Nakayama and S. Shimojo, Da vinci stereopsis: Depth and subjective occluding contours from unpaired image points, Vision Research, vol.30, issue.11, pp.1811-1825, 1990.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(90)90161-D

P. Neri, H. Bridge, and D. Heeger, Stereoscopic Processing of Absolute and Relative Disparity in Human Visual Cortex, Journal of Neurophysiology, vol.92, issue.3, pp.1880-1891, 2004.
DOI : 10.1152/jn.01042.2003

F. Newell, P. Mamassian, and D. Alais, Multisensory Processing in Review: from Physiology to Behaviour, Seeing and perceiving, vol.23, issue.1, pp.3-38, 2010.

H. Nienborg and B. G. Cumming, Macaque V2 Neurons, But Not V1 Neurons, Show Choice-Related Activity, Journal of Neuroscience, vol.26, issue.37, pp.26-9567, 2006.
DOI : 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2256-06.2006

H. Nienborg, H. Bridge, A. J. Parker, and B. G. Cumming, Receptive Field Size in V1 Neurons Limits Acuity for Perceiving Disparity Modulation, Journal of Neuroscience, vol.24, issue.9, pp.2065-2076, 2004.
DOI : 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3887-03.2004

A. M. Norcia and C. W. Tyler, Temporal frequency limits for stereoscopic apparent motion processes, Vision Research, vol.24, issue.5, pp.395-401, 1984.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(84)90037-3

K. N. Ogle, On the limits of stereoscopic vision., Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol.44, issue.4, pp.253-259, 1952.
DOI : 10.1037/h0057643

K. N. Ogle and M. P. Weil, Stereoscopic Vision and the Duration of the Stimulus, Archives of Ophthalmology, vol.59, issue.1, pp.4-17, 1958.
DOI : 10.1001/archopht.1958.00940020028002

I. Ohzawa, G. C. Deangelis, and R. D. Freeman, Encoding of binocular disparity by complex cells in the cat's visual cortex, Journal of Neurophysiology, vol.77, issue.6, pp.2879-2909, 1997.

H. Ono, L. Lillakas, P. M. Grove, and M. Suzuki, Leonardo's constraint: Two opaque objects cannot be seen in the same direction., Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, vol.132, issue.2, pp.253-265, 2003.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-3445.132.2.253

H. Ono, K. Shimono, and K. Shibuta, Occlusion as a depth cue in the Wheatstone-Panum limiting case, Perception & Psychophysics, vol.128, issue.1, pp.3-13, 1992.
DOI : 10.3758/BF03205069

H. Ono, N. J. Wade, and L. Lillakas, The Pursuit of Leonardo's Constraint, Perception, vol.128, issue.1, pp.31-83, 2002.
DOI : 10.1068/p3079

T. Otto and P. Mamassian, Noise and Correlations in Parallel Perceptual Decision Making, Current Biology, pp.1-6, 2012.

P. L. Panum and P. L. , Untersuchungen uber das Sehen mit Zwei Augen, 1858.

A. J. Parker, Binocular depth perception and the cerebral cortex, Nature Reviews Neuroscience, vol.87, issue.5, pp.379-391, 2007.
DOI : 10.1038/nrn2131

D. G. Pelli, The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies, Spatial Vision, vol.10, issue.4, pp.437-442, 1997.
DOI : 10.1163/156856897X00366

J. D. Pettigrew, T. Nikara, and P. O. Bishop, Binocular interaction on single units in cat striate cortex: Simultaneous stimulation by single moving slit with receptive fields in correspondence, Experimental Brain Research, vol.6, issue.4, pp.391-410, 1968.
DOI : 10.1007/BF00233186

M. J. Pianta and B. J. Gillam, Paired and unpaired features can be equally effective in human depth perception, Vision Research, vol.43, issue.1, pp.1-6, 2003.
DOI : 10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00399-1

M. J. Pianta and B. J. Gillam, Monocular gap stereopsis: manipulation of the outer edge disparity and the shape of the gap, Vision Research, vol.43, issue.18, pp.1937-1950, 2003.
DOI : 10.1016/S0042-6989(03)00252-9

G. F. Poggio and W. H. Talbot, Mechanisms of static and dynamic stereopsis in foveal cortex of the rhesus monkey, The Journal of Physiology, vol.315, issue.1, pp.469-492, 1981.
DOI : 10.1113/jphysiol.1981.sp013759

G. F. Poggio, B. C. Motter, S. Squatrito, and Y. Trotter, Responses of neurons in visual cortex (V1 and V2) of the alert macaque to dynamic random-dot stereograms, Vision Research, vol.25, issue.3, pp.397-406, 1985.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(85)90065-3

S. B. Pollard, J. E. Mayhew, and J. P. Frisby, PMF: A Stereo Correspondence Algorithm Using a Disparity Gradient Limit, Perception, vol.181, issue.4, pp.449-470, 1985.
DOI : 10.1068/p140449

C. Portfors-yeomans and D. Regan, Cyclopean Discrimination Thresholds for the Direction and Speed of Motion in Depth, Vision Research, vol.36, issue.20, pp.3265-3279, 1996.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(96)00065-X

T. J. Preston, Z. Kourtzi, and A. E. Welchman, Adaptive Estimation of Three-Dimensional Structure in the Human Brain, Journal of Neuroscience, vol.29, issue.6, pp.1688-1698, 2009.
DOI : 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5021-08.2009

T. J. Preston, S. Li, Z. Kourtzi, and A. E. Welchman, Multivoxel Pattern Selectivity for Perceptually Relevant Binocular Disparities in the Human Brain, Journal of Neuroscience, vol.28, issue.44, pp.28-11315, 2008.
DOI : 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2728-08.2008

S. Polyak, The Vertebrate Visual System, 1957.

C. Rashbass and G. Westheimer, Disjunctive eye movements, The Journal of Physiology, vol.159, issue.2, pp.339-360, 1961.
DOI : 10.1113/jphysiol.1961.sp006812

G. H. Recanzone, Auditory Influences on Visual Temporal Rate Perception, Journal of Neurophysiology, vol.89, issue.2, pp.1078-1093, 2003.
DOI : 10.1152/jn.00706.2002

D. Regan, Binocular correlates of the direction of motion in depth, Vision Research, vol.33, issue.16, pp.2359-2360, 1993.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(93)90114-C

D. Regan and K. Beverley, Disparity Detectors in Human Depth Perception: Evidence for Directional Selectivity, Science, vol.181, issue.4102, pp.181-877, 1973.
DOI : 10.1126/science.181.4102.877

D. Regan and K. I. Beverley, Electrophysiological Evidence for Existence of Neurones sensitive to Direction of Depth Movement, Nature, vol.13, issue.5434, pp.246-504, 1973.
DOI : 10.1038/246504a0

B. Rokers, L. K. Cormack, and A. C. Huk, Strong percepts of motion through depth without strong percepts of position in depth, Journal of Vision, vol.8, issue.4, pp.6-6, 2008.
DOI : 10.1167/8.4.6.M2

B. Rokers, L. K. Cormack, and A. C. Huk, Disparity- and velocity-based signals for three-dimensional motion perception in human MT+, Nature Neuroscience, vol.7, issue.8, pp.12-1050, 2009.
DOI : 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00452-4

B. Rokers, T. B. Czuba, L. K. Cormack, and A. C. Huk, Motion processing with two eyes in three dimensions, Journal of Vision, vol.11, issue.2, pp.10-10, 2011.
DOI : 10.1167/11.2.10

N. Sadato, A. Pascual-leone, J. Grafman, V. Ibañez, M. P. Deiber et al., Activation of the primary visual cortex by Braille reading in blind subjects, Nature, vol.380, issue.6574, pp.380-526, 1996.
DOI : 10.1038/380526a0

J. M. Samonds, B. R. Potetz, and T. S. Lee, Cooperative and Competitive Interactions Facilitate Stereo Computations in Macaque Primary Visual Cortex, Journal of Neuroscience, vol.29, issue.50, pp.29-15780, 2009.
DOI : 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2305-09.2009

C. M. Schor and C. W. Tyler, Spatio-temporal properties of Panum's fusional area, Vision Research, vol.21, issue.5, pp.683-692, 1981.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(81)90076-6

C. E. Schroeder and J. J. Foxe, The timing and laminar profile of converging inputs to multisensory areas of the macaque neocortex, Cognitive Brain Research, vol.14, issue.1, pp.187-198, 2002.
DOI : 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00073-3

A. R. Seitz, R. Kim, and L. Shams, Sound Facilitates Visual Learning, Current Biology, vol.16, issue.14, pp.1422-1427, 2006.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cub.2006.05.048

I. Serrano-pedraza, A specialization for vertical disparity discontinuities, Journal of Vision, vol.10, issue.3, pp.1-25, 2010.
DOI : 10.1167/10.3.2

L. Shams, Y. Kamitani, and S. Shimojo, Illusions. What you see is what you hear, Nature, issue.6814, pp.408-788, 2000.

L. Shams, Y. Kamitani, and S. Shimojo, Visual illusion induced by sound, Cognitive Brain Research, vol.14, issue.1, pp.147-152, 2002.
DOI : 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00069-1

J. Sheedy, I. Bailey, M. Buri, and E. Bass, Binocular vs. Monocular Task Performance, Optometry and Vision Science, vol.63, issue.10, pp.63-839, 1986.
DOI : 10.1097/00006324-198610000-00008

S. Shioiri, D. Kakehi, T. Tashiro, and H. Yaguchi, Integration of monocular motion signals and the analysis of interocular velocity differences for the perception of motion-in-depth, Journal of Vision, vol.9, issue.13, pp.1-17, 2009.
DOI : 10.1167/9.13.10

S. Shioiri, H. Saisho, and H. Yaguchi, Motion in depth based on inter-ocular velocity differences, Vision Research, vol.40, issue.19, pp.2565-2572, 2000.
DOI : 10.1016/S0042-6989(00)00130-9

W. Spileers, G. A. Orban, B. Gulyas, and H. Maes, Selectivity of cat area 18 neurons for direction and speed in depth, Journal of Neurophysiology, vol.63, issue.4, pp.936-954, 1990.

R. A. Stevenson and T. W. James, Audiovisual integration in human superior temporal sulcus: Inverse effectiveness and the neural processing of speech and object recognition, NeuroImage, vol.44, issue.3, pp.1210-1223, 2009.
DOI : 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.034

L. C. Thomson, Binocular summation within the nervous pathways of the pupillary light reflex, The Journal of Physiology, vol.106, issue.1, pp.59-65, 1947.
DOI : 10.1113/jphysiol.1947.sp004192

F. Tong, Competing theories of binocular rivalry: A possible resolution, Brain and Mind, vol.2, issue.1, pp.55-83, 2001.
DOI : 10.1023/A:1017942718744

F. Tong, M. Meng, and R. Blake, Neural bases of binocular rivalry, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol.10, issue.11, pp.502-511, 2006.
DOI : 10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.003

F. Tong, K. Nakayama, J. T. Vaughan, and N. Kanwisher, Binocular Rivalry and Visual Awareness in Human Extrastriate Cortex, Neuron, vol.21, issue.4, pp.753-759, 1998.
DOI : 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80592-9

A. Treisman, Binocular rivalry and stereoscopic depth perception, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol.16, issue.1, pp.23-37, 1962.
DOI : 10.1111/j.1755-3768.1956.tb03340.x

H. P. Trivedi and S. A. Lloyd, The Role of Disparity Gradient in Stereo Vision, Perception, vol.14, issue.6, pp.685-690, 1985.
DOI : 10.1068/p140685

D. Tsao, B. Conway, and M. Livingstone, Receptive Fields of Disparity-Tuned Simple Cells in Macaque V1, Neuron, vol.38, issue.1, pp.103-114, 2003.
DOI : 10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00150-8

C. W. Tyler, Stereoscopic Depth Movement: Two Eyes Less Sensitive than One, Science, vol.174, issue.4012, pp.958-961, 1971.
DOI : 10.1126/science.174.4012.958

C. W. Tyler, Stereoscopic Vision: Cortical Limitations and a Disparity Scaling Effect, Science, vol.181, issue.4096, pp.181-276, 1973.
DOI : 10.1126/science.181.4096.276

C. W. Tyler, Spatial organization of binocular disparity sensitivity, Vision Research, vol.15, issue.5, pp.583-590, 1975.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(75)90306-5

T. Uka, S. Tanabe, M. Watanabe, and I. Fujita, Neural Correlates of Fine Depth Discrimination in Monkey Inferior Temporal Cortex, Journal of Neuroscience, vol.25, issue.46, pp.25-10796, 2005.
DOI : 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1637-05.2005

W. R. Uttal, N. S. Davis, and C. Welke, Stereoscopic perception with brief exposures, Perception & Psychophysics, vol.15, issue.5, pp.599-604, 1994.
DOI : 10.3758/BF03206955

W. R. Uttal, J. Fitzgerald, and T. E. Eskin, Parameters of tachistoscopic stereopsis, Vision Research, vol.15, issue.6, pp.705-712, 1975.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(75)90288-6

R. Van-ee, Stochastic variations in sensory awareness are driven by noisy neuronal adaptation: evidence from serial correlations in perceptual bistability, Journal of the Optical Society of America A, vol.26, issue.12, pp.2612-2622, 2009.
DOI : 10.1364/JOSAA.26.002612

P. A. Warren, L. T. Maloney, and M. S. Landy, Interpolating sampled contours in 3-D: analyses of variability and bias, Vision Research, vol.42, issue.21, pp.42-2431, 2002.
DOI : 10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00266-3

P. A. Warren, L. T. Maloney, and M. S. Landy, Interpolating sampled contours in 3D: perturbation analyses, Vision Research, vol.44, issue.8, pp.815-832, 2004.
DOI : 10.1016/j.visres.2003.11.007

O. Watanabe and K. Fukushima, Stereo algorithm that extracts a depth cue from interocularly unpaired points, Neural networks : the official journal of the International Neural Network Society, pp.4-5, 1999.
DOI : 10.1016/S0893-6080(99)00019-2

G. Westheimer, Spatial interaction in the domain of disparity signals in human stereoscopic vision., The Journal of Physiology, vol.370, issue.1, pp.619-629, 1986.
DOI : 10.1113/jphysiol.1986.sp015954

L. M. Wilcox and D. C. Lakra, Depth from binocular half-occlusions in stereoscopic images of natural scenes, Perception, vol.36, issue.6, pp.36-830, 2007.
DOI : 10.1068/p5708

H. R. Wilson, Computational evidence for a rivalry hierarchy in vision, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol.100, issue.24, pp.14499-14503, 2003.
DOI : 10.1073/pnas.2333622100

M. Zannoli, J. Cass, P. Mamassian, and D. Alais, Synchronized Audio-Visual Transients Drive Efficient Visual Search for Motion-in-Depth, PLoS ONE, vol.10, issue.5, p.37190, 2012.
DOI : 10.1371/journal.pone.0037190.s003

S. M. Zeki, Cells responding to changing image size and disparity in the cortex of the rhesus monkey, The Journal of Physiology, vol.242, issue.3, pp.827-841, 1974.
DOI : 10.1113/jphysiol.1974.sp010736