D. Alais and D. Burr, The Ventriloquist Effect Results from Near-Optimal Bimodal Integration, Current Biology, vol.14, issue.3, pp.257-262, 2004.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cub.2004.01.029

J. C. Alvarado, J. W. Vaughan, T. R. Stanford, and B. E. Stein, Multisensory Versus Unisensory Integration: Contrasting Modes in the Superior Colliculus, Journal of Neurophysiology, vol.97, issue.5, 2007.
DOI : 10.1152/jn.00018.2007

S. E. Asch and H. A. Witkin, Studies in space orientation: I. Perception of the upright with displaced visual fields., Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol.38, issue.3, pp.325-337, 1948.
DOI : 10.1037/h0057855

S. E. Asch and H. A. Witkin, Studies in space orientation. II. Perception of the upright with displaced visual fields and with body tilted., Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol.38, issue.4, pp.455-477, 1948.
DOI : 10.1037/h0054121

H. Aubert, Eine scheinbare bedeutende Drehung von Objecten bei Neigung des Kopfes nach rechts oder links, Archiv f??r Pathologische Anatomie und Physiologie und f??r Klinische Medicin, vol.20, issue.3-4, pp.381-393, 1861.
DOI : 10.1007/BF02355256

M. Avillac, S. Deneve, E. Olivier, and A. Pouget, Reference frames for representing visual and tactile locations in parietal cortex, Nature Neuroscience, vol.140, issue.7, pp.941-949, 2005.
DOI : 10.1038/nn1480

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00314588

J. Barra, A. Marquer, R. Joassin, C. Reymond, L. Metge et al., Humans use internal models to construct and update a sense of verticality, Brain, vol.133, issue.12, pp.3552-3563, 2010.
DOI : 10.1093/brain/awq311

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00775946

P. W. Battaglia, R. A. Jacobs, and R. N. Aslin, Bayesian integration of visual and auditory signals for spatial localization, Journal of the Optical Society of America A, vol.20, issue.7, pp.1391-1397, 2003.
DOI : 10.1364/JOSAA.20.001391

U. R. Beierholm, S. R. Quartz, and L. Shams, Bayesian priors are encoded independently from likelihoods in human multisensory perception, Journal of Vision, vol.9, issue.5, pp.23-29, 2009.
DOI : 10.1167/9.5.23

D. Bernardin, B. Isableu, P. Fourcade, and B. G. Bardy, Differential exploitation of the inertia tensor in multi-joint arm reaching, Experimental Brain Research, vol.311, issue.6, pp.487-495, 2005.
DOI : 10.1007/s00221-005-0161-4

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00584570

A. Berthoz, Le sens du mouvement, p.242, 1997.

J. M. Bland and D. G. Altman, Statistical methods for assessing agreement between two methods of clinical measurement, International Journal of Nursing Studies, vol.47, issue.8, pp.307-310, 1986.
DOI : 10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2009.10.001

O. Bock, Effects of a tilted visual background on human sensory-motor coordination, Experimental Brain Research, vol.115, issue.3, pp.507-512, 1997.
DOI : 10.1007/PL00005720

J. L. Bradshaw and N. C. Nettleton, The nature of hemispheric specialization in man, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, vol.25, issue.01, pp.51-63, 1981.
DOI : 10.1037/h0065432

T. Brandt, P. Bartenstein, A. Janek, and M. Dieterich, Reciprocal inhibitory visual-vestibular interaction. Visual motion stimulation deactivates the parieto-insular vestibular cortex, Brain, vol.121, issue.9, pp.121-1749, 1998.
DOI : 10.1093/brain/121.9.1749

A. Bray, A. Subanandan, B. Isableu, T. Ohlmann, J. F. Golding et al., We are most aware of our place in the world when about to fall, Current Biology, vol.14, issue.15, pp.609-610, 2004.
DOI : 10.1016/j.cub.2004.07.040

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00580058

F. Brenet and M. Luyat, Frame anisotropy : a problem of individual validity Ref Type: Personal Communication, 1995.

F. Brenet, T. Ohlmann, and C. Marendaz, Interaction vision/posture lors de la localisation d'une cible enchâssée, Bulletin de Psychologie, vol.388, pp.22-30, 1988.

J. P. Bresciani, F. Dammeier, and M. O. Ernst, Vision and touch are automatically integrated for the perception of sequences of events, Journal of Vision, vol.6, issue.5, pp.554-564, 2006.
DOI : 10.1167/6.5.2

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00947239

J. P. Bresciani and M. O. Ernst, Signal reliability modulates auditory???tactile integration for event counting, NeuroReport, vol.18, issue.11, pp.1157-1161, 2007.
DOI : 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3281ace0ca

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00947238

J. P. Bresciani, M. O. Ernst, K. Drewing, G. Bouyer, V. Maury et al., Feeling what you hear: auditory signals can modulate tactile tap perception, Experimental Brain Research, vol.67, issue.2, pp.172-180, 2005.
DOI : 10.1007/s00221-004-2128-2

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00342908

L. Bringoux, C. Bourdin, J. C. Lepecq, P. M. Sandor, J. M. Pergandi et al., Interaction between Reference Frames during Subjective Vertical Estimates in a Tilted Immersive Virtual Environment, Perception, vol.54, issue.7, pp.1053-1071, 2009.
DOI : 10.1068/p6089

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01436924

L. Bringoux, V. Nougier, P. A. Barraud, L. Marin, and C. Raphel, Contribution of somesthetic information to the perception of body orientation in the pitch dimension, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, vol.65, issue.5, pp.909-923, 2003.
DOI : 10.1007/s002219900238

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00193896

L. Bringoux, G. Robic, G. M. Gauthier, and J. L. Vercher, Judging beforehand the possibility of passing under obstacles without motion: the influence of egocentric and geocentric frames of reference, Experimental Brain Research, vol.58, issue.1, pp.673-680, 2008.
DOI : 10.1007/s00221-007-1194-7

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-01436033

A. M. Bronstein, Suppression of visually evoked postural responses, Experimental Brain Research, vol.239, issue.3, pp.655-658, 1986.
DOI : 10.1007/BF00237488

S. Carver, T. Kiemel, and J. J. Jeka, Modeling the Dynamics of Sensory Reweighting, Biological Cybernetics, vol.23, issue.2, pp.123-134, 2006.
DOI : 10.1007/s00422-006-0069-5

S. Carver, T. Kiemel, H. Van-der-kooij, and J. J. Jeka, Comparing internal models of the dynamics of the visual environment, Biological Cybernetics, vol.23, issue.3, pp.147-163, 2005.
DOI : 10.1007/s00422-004-0535-x

H. Ceyte, C. Cian, M. Trousselard, and P. A. Barraud, Influence of perceived egocentric coordinates on the subjective visual vertical, Neuroscience Letters, vol.462, issue.1, pp.85-88, 2009.
DOI : 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.06.048

C. Cian, D. Esquivie, P. A. Barraud, and C. Raphel, Respective Contribution of Orientation Contrast and Illusion of Self-Tilt to the Rod-And-Frame Effect, Perception, vol.79, issue.6, pp.623-630, 1995.
DOI : 10.1068/p240623

B. Clark and A. Graybiel, Visual perception of the horizontal following exposure to radial acceleration on a centrifuge., Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, vol.44, issue.6, pp.525-534, 1951.
DOI : 10.1037/h0059470

B. Clark and A. Graybiel, Visual perception of the horizontal during prolonged exposure to radial acceleration on a centrifuge., Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol.63, issue.3, pp.294-301, 1962.
DOI : 10.1037/h0039752

B. Clark and A. Graybiel, Influence of Contact Cues on the Perception of the Oculogravic Illusion, Acta Oto-Laryngologica, vol.42, issue.1-6, pp.373-380, 1968.
DOI : 10.3109/00016486809120978

J. O. Coq and C. Xerri, Sensorimotor experience modulates age-dependent alterations of the forepaw representation in the rat primary somatosensory cortex, Neuroscience, vol.104, issue.3, pp.705-715, 2001.
DOI : 10.1016/S0306-4522(01)00123-3

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00095473

C. Darlot, The cerebellum as a predictor of neural messages???I. The stable estimator hypothesis, Neuroscience, vol.56, issue.3, pp.617-646, 1993.
DOI : 10.1016/0306-4522(93)90361-I

B. L. Day and J. Cole, Vestibular-evoked postural responses in the absence of somatosensory information, Brain, vol.125, issue.9, pp.2081-2088, 2002.
DOI : 10.1093/brain/awf212

D. Vrijer, M. Medendorp, W. P. Van-gisbergen, and J. A. , Accuracy-precision trade-off in visual orientation constancy, Journal of Vision, vol.9, issue.2, pp.9-15, 2009.
DOI : 10.1167/9.2.9

S. Deneve and A. Pouget, Basis Functions for Object-Centered Representations, Neuron, vol.37, issue.2, pp.347-359, 2003.
DOI : 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)01184-4

S. Deneve and A. Pouget, Bayesian multisensory integration and cross-modal spatial links, Journal of Physiology-Paris, vol.98, issue.1-3, pp.249-258, 2004.
DOI : 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2004.03.011

A. Deutschlander, S. Bense, T. Stephan, M. Schwaiger, T. Brandt et al., Sensory system interactions during simultaneous vestibular and visual stimulation in PET, Human Brain Mapping, vol.11, issue.Suppl, pp.92-103, 2002.
DOI : 10.1002/hbm.10030

M. Dieterich and T. Brandt, Brain activation studies on visual-vestibular and ocular motor interaction, Current Opinion in Neurology, vol.13, issue.1, pp.13-18, 2000.
DOI : 10.1097/00019052-200002000-00004

J. Driver and C. Spence, Crossmodal attention, Current Opinion in Neurobiology, vol.8, issue.2, pp.245-253, 1998.
DOI : 10.1016/S0959-4388(98)80147-5

S. M. Ebenholtz, Depth separation fails to modulate the orientation-inhibition effect, Perception & Psychophysics, vol.38, issue.6, pp.533-535, 1985.
DOI : 10.3758/BF03204918

S. M. Ebenholtz and J. W. Callan, Tilt adaptation as a feedback control process., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.6, issue.3, 1980.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.6.3.413

S. M. Ebenholtz and G. W. Glaser, Absence of depth processing in the large-frame rod-and-frame effect, Perception & Psychophysics, vol.38, issue.2, pp.134-140, 1982.
DOI : 10.3758/BF03204272

M. Eimer and J. Driver, Crossmodal links in endogenous and exogenous spatial attention: evidence from event-related brain potential studies, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, vol.25, issue.6, pp.497-511, 2001.
DOI : 10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00029-X

M. O. Ernst and M. S. Banks, Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion, Nature, vol.415, issue.6870, pp.429-433, 2002.
DOI : 10.1038/415429a

M. O. Ernst, M. S. Banks, and H. H. Bulthoff, Touch can change visual slant perception, Nature Neuroscience, vol.3, issue.1, pp.69-73, 2000.
DOI : 10.1038/71140

M. O. Ernst and H. H. Bulthoff, Merging the senses into a robust percept, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol.8, issue.4, pp.162-169, 2004.
DOI : 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.002

J. L. Fleiss, Reliability of Measurement, Design and Analysis of Clinical Experiments, pp.1-32, 1986.
DOI : 10.1002/9781118032923.ch1

B. Fourre, B. Isableu, D. Bernardin, M. Gueguen, G. Giraudet et al., The role of body centre of mass on haptic subjective vertical, Neuroscience Letters, vol.465, issue.3, pp.230-234, 2009.
DOI : 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.07.005

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00579441

M. A. Frens, A. J. Van-opstal, and R. F. Van-der-willigen, Spatial and temporal factors determine auditory-visual interactions in human saccadic eye movements, Perception & Psychophysics, vol.2, issue.6, pp.802-816, 1995.
DOI : 10.3758/BF03206796

N. P. Friedman, A. Miyake, S. E. Young, J. C. Defries, R. P. Corley et al., Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin., Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, vol.137, issue.2, pp.201-225, 2008.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.201

J. J. Gibson, The ecological approach to visual perception, 1979.

D. R. Goodenough, P. W. Cox, E. Sigman, and W. E. Strawderman, A cognitive style conception of the field dependence dimension, Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, vol.5, pp.687-705, 1985.

D. R. Goodenough, A. Nowak, P. K. Oltman, P. W. Cox, and E. Sigman, A visually induced illusion of body tilt in a horizontal plane, Perception & Psychophysics, vol.38, issue.3, pp.268-272, 1982.
DOI : 10.3758/BF03202533

D. R. Goodenough, P. K. Oltman, E. Sigman, and P. W. Cox, The rod-and-frame illusion in erect and supine observers, Perception & Psychophysics, vol.25, issue.4, pp.365-370, 1981.
DOI : 10.3758/BF03207346

D. R. Goodenough, E. Sigman, P. K. Oltman, J. Rosso, and H. Mertz, Eye torsion in response to a tilted visual stimulus, Vision Research, vol.19, issue.10, pp.1177-1179, 1979.
DOI : 10.1016/0042-6989(79)90015-4

A. Graybiel, OCULOGRAVIC ILLUSION, Archives of Ophthalmology, vol.48, issue.5, pp.605-615, 1952.
DOI : 10.1001/archopht.1952.00920010616007

M. Guerraz, M. Luyat, D. Poquin, and T. Ohlmann, The role of neck afferents in subjective orientation in the visual and tactile sensory modalities, Acta Otolaryngol, vol.120, pp.735-738, 2000.
URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00332322

M. Guerraz, D. Poquin, M. Luyat, and T. Ohlmann, HEAD ORIENTATION INVOLVEMENT IN ASSESSMENT OF THE SUBJECTIVE VERTICAL DURING WHOLE BODY TILT, Perceptual and Motor Skills, vol.83, issue.2, pp.643-648, 1998.
DOI : 10.3758/BF03212730

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00332325

M. Guerraz, D. Poquin, and T. Ohlmann, The role of head-centric spatial reference with a static and kinetic visual disturbance, Perception & Psychophysics, vol.48, issue.2, pp.287-295, 1998.
DOI : 10.3758/BF03206037

M. Guerraz, L. Yardley, P. Bertholon, L. Pollak, P. Rudge et al., Visual vertigo: symptom assessment, spatial orientation and postural control, Brain, vol.124, issue.8, pp.1646-1656, 2001.
DOI : 10.1093/brain/124.8.1646

S. Guest and C. Spence, What role does multisensory integration play in the visuotactile perception of texture?, International Journal of Psychophysiology, vol.50, issue.1-2, pp.63-80, 2003.
DOI : 10.1016/S0167-8760(03)00125-9

E. P. Hanavan and . Jr, A mathematical model of human body, AMRL.TR, pp.1-149, 1964.
DOI : 10.1037/e400822004-001

H. B. Helbig and M. O. Ernst, Optimal integration of shape information from vision and touch, Experimental Brain Research, vol.63, issue.4, pp.595-606, 2007.
DOI : 10.1007/s00221-006-0814-y

H. B. Helbig and M. O. Ernst, Visual-haptic cue weighting is independent of modality-specific attention, Journal of Vision, vol.8, issue.1, pp.21-37, 2008.
DOI : 10.1167/8.1.21

J. M. Hillis, M. O. Ernst, M. S. Banks, and M. S. Landy, Combining Sensory Information: Mandatory Fusion Within, but Not Between, Senses, Science, vol.298, issue.5598, pp.1627-1630, 2002.
DOI : 10.1126/science.1075396

URL : http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.278.6134

I. P. Howard, Human visual orientation, 1982.

I. P. Howard, The perception of posture, self motion, and the visual vertical, Handbook of Perception and Human Performance, 1986.

I. P. Howard and L. Childerson, The Contribution of Motion, the Visual Frame, and Visual Polarity to Sensations of Body Tilt, Perception, vol.2, issue.7, pp.753-762, 1994.
DOI : 10.1068/p230753

I. P. Howard and G. Hu, Visually Induced Reorientation Illusions, Perception, vol.46, issue.7, pp.583-600, 2001.
DOI : 10.1068/p3106

M. Huteau, Style cognitif et personnalité : la dépendance-indépendance à l'égard du champ, 1987.

J. T. Inglis, P. M. Kennedy, C. Wells, and R. Chua, The Role of Cutaneous Receptors in the Foot, Adv.Exp.Med.Biol, vol.508, pp.111-117, 2002.
DOI : 10.1007/978-1-4615-0713-0_14

B. Isableu, T. Ohlmann, J. Cremieux, and B. Amblard, Selection of spatial frame of reference and postural control variability, Experimental Brain Research, vol.114, issue.3, pp.584-589, 1997.
DOI : 10.1007/PL00005667

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00580059

B. Isableu, T. Ohlmann, J. Cremieux, and B. Amblard, Differential approach to strategies of segmental stabilisation in postural control, Experimental Brain Research, vol.26, issue.2, pp.208-221, 2003.
DOI : 10.1016/S0167-9457(02)00077-5

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00580057

B. Isableu, T. Ohlmann, J. Cremieux, N. Vuillerme, B. Amblard et al., Individual differences in the ability to identify, select and use appropriate frames of reference for perceptuo-motor control, Neuroscience, vol.169, issue.3, pp.1199-1215, 2010.
DOI : 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.05.072

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00580045

B. Isableu and N. Vuillerme, Differential integration of kinaesthetic signals to postural control, Experimental Brain Research, vol.311, issue.4, pp.763-768, 2006.
DOI : 10.1007/s00221-006-0630-4

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00580050

Y. Ito and M. A. Gresty, Shift of subjective reference and visual orientation during slow pitch tilt for the seated human subject, Brain Research Bulletin, vol.40, issue.5-6, pp.417-421, 1996.
DOI : 10.1016/0361-9230(96)00136-0

Y. Ito and M. A. Gresty, Subjective postural orientation and visual vertical during slow pitch tilt for the seated human subject, Aviat.Space Environ.Med, vol.68, pp.3-12, 1997.

J. Jeka, K. S. Oie, and T. Kiemel, Multisensory information for human postural control: integrating touch and vision, Experimental Brain Research, vol.134, issue.1, pp.107-125, 2000.
DOI : 10.1007/s002210000412

R. G. Kaptein and J. A. Van-gisbergen, Nature of the Transition Between Two Modes of External Space Perception in Tilted Subjects, Journal of Neurophysiology, vol.93, issue.6, pp.3356-3369, 2005.
DOI : 10.1152/jn.01137.2004

N. Kato, A fundamental study of the Rod-and-Frame Test, Japanese psychological Research, vol.7, pp.61-88, 1965.

P. M. Kennedy and J. T. Inglis, Distribution and behaviour of glabrous cutaneous receptors in the human foot sole, The Journal of Physiology, vol.34, issue.3, pp.995-1002, 2002.
DOI : 10.1113/jphysiol.2001.013087

S. Kennett, M. Eimer, C. Spence, and J. Driver, Tactile-Visual Links in Exogenous Spatial Attention under Different Postures: Convergent Evidence from Psychophysics and ERPs, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, vol.9, issue.4, pp.462-478, 2001.
DOI : 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb03209.x

T. Kiemel, K. S. Oie, and J. J. Jeka, Multisensory fusion and the stochastic structure of postural sway, Biological Cybernetics, vol.87, issue.4, pp.262-277, 2002.
DOI : 10.1007/s00422-002-0333-2

J. Kluzik, F. B. Horak, and R. J. Peterka, Differences in preferred reference frames for postural orientation shown by after-effects of stance on an inclined surface, Experimental Brain Research, vol.51, issue.4, pp.474-489, 2005.
DOI : 10.1007/s00221-004-2124-6

D. C. Knill and A. Pouget, The Bayesian brain: the role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation, Trends in Neurosciences, vol.27, issue.12, pp.712-719, 2004.
DOI : 10.1016/j.tins.2004.10.007

K. P. Kording and D. M. Wolpert, Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning, Nature, vol.427, issue.6971, pp.244-247, 2004.
DOI : 10.1038/nature02169

K. P. Kording and D. M. Wolpert, The loss function of sensorimotor learning, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol.101, issue.26, pp.9839-9842, 2004.
DOI : 10.1073/pnas.0308394101

K. P. Kording and D. M. Wolpert, Bayesian decision theory in sensorimotor control, Trends in Cognitive Sciences, vol.10, issue.7, pp.319-326, 2006.
DOI : 10.1016/j.tics.2006.05.003

Y. Kwon, Flexibility of the experimental simulation approach to the analysis of human airborne movements: body segment parameter estimation, International Research in Sport Biomechanics, pp.44-55, 2002.

J. R. Lackner and P. Dizio, Artificial gravity as a countermeasure in long-duration space flight, Journal of Neuroscience Research, vol.871, issue.2, pp.169-176, 2000.
DOI : 10.1002/1097-4547(20001015)62:2<169::AID-JNR2>3.0.CO;2-B

E. Ladavas, F. Pavani, and A. Farne, Auditory peripersonal space in humans: a case of auditory-tactile extinction, Neurocase, vol.7, issue.2, pp.97-103, 2001.
DOI : 10.1093/neucas/7.2.103

S. J. Lederman, G. Thorne, and B. Jones, Perception of texture by vision and touch: Multidimensionality and intersensory integration., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.12, issue.2, 1986.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.12.2.169

D. N. Lee and E. Aronson, Visual proprioceptive control of standing in human infants, Perception & Psychophysics, vol.2, issue.3, pp.529-532, 1974.
DOI : 10.3758/BF03199297

D. N. Lee and R. Lishman, Visual porprioceptive control of stance, Journal of Human Movement Study, vol.1, pp.87-95, 1975.

C. Lopez, M. Lacour, and L. Borel, Perception de la verticalité et représentations spatiales dans les aires corticales vestibulaires, 2005.

C. Lopez, M. Lacour, J. Magnan, and L. Borel, Visual field dependence???independence before and after unilateral vestibular loss, NeuroReport, vol.17, issue.8, pp.797-803, 2006.
DOI : 10.1097/01.wnr.0000221843.58373.c8

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00095234

M. Luyat, E. Gentaz, T. R. Corte, and M. Guerraz, Reference frames and haptic perception of orientation: Body and head tilt effects on the oblique effect, Perception & Psychophysics, vol.37, issue.3, pp.541-554, 2001.
DOI : 10.3758/BF03194419

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00332316

M. Luyat, T. Ohlmann, and P. A. Barraud, Subjective vertical and postural activity, Acta Psychologica, vol.95, issue.2, pp.181-193, 1997.
DOI : 10.1016/S0001-6918(96)00015-7

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00332329

E. Macaluso and J. Driver, Spatial attention and crossmodal interactions between vision and touch, Neuropsychologia, vol.39, issue.12, pp.1304-1316, 2001.
DOI : 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00119-1

P. R. Macneilage, M. S. Banks, D. R. Berger, and H. H. Bulthoff, A Bayesian model of the disambiguation of gravitoinertial force by visual cues, Experimental Brain Research, vol.86, issue.3, pp.263-290, 2007.
DOI : 10.1007/s00221-006-0792-0

C. Marendaz, Selection of Reference Frames and the ???Vicariance??? of Perceptual Systems, Perception, vol.38, issue.6, pp.739-751, 1989.
DOI : 10.1068/p180739

J. B. Mattingley, J. Driver, N. Beschin, and I. H. Robertson, Attentional competition between modalities: extinction between touch and vision after right hemisphere damage, Neuropsychologia, vol.35, issue.6, pp.867-880, 1997.
DOI : 10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00008-0

C. Maurer, T. Mergner, B. Bolha, and F. Hlavacka, Vestibular, visual, and somatosensory contributions to human control of upright stance, Neuroscience Letters, vol.281, issue.2-3, pp.99-102, 2000.
DOI : 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)00814-4

C. Maurer and R. J. Peterka, A New Interpretation of Spontaneous Sway Measures Based on a Simple Model of Human Postural Control, Journal of Neurophysiology, vol.93, issue.1, pp.189-200, 2005.
DOI : 10.1152/jn.00221.2004

L. M. Mcguire and P. N. Sabes, Sensory transformations and the use of multiple reference frames for reach planning, Nature Neuroscience, vol.62, issue.8, pp.1056-1061, 2009.
DOI : 10.1152/jn.00021.2005

L. B. Merabet, R. Hamilton, G. Schlaug, J. D. Swisher, E. T. Kiriakopoulos et al., Rapid and Reversible Recruitment of Early Visual Cortex for Touch, PLoS ONE, vol.112, issue.1, p.3046, 2008.
DOI : 10.1371/journal.pone.0003046.t001

M. A. Meredith, J. W. Nemitz, and B. E. Stein, Determinants of multisensory integration in superior colliculus neurons. I. Temporal factors, J.Neurosci, vol.7, pp.3215-3229, 1987.

H. Mittelstaedt, A new solution to the problem of the subjective vertical, Naturwissenschaften, vol.1, issue.3, pp.272-281, 1983.
DOI : 10.1007/BF00404833

G. E. Muller, Uber das Aubertsche Phänomen, Zeitschrift für Sinnesphysiologie, vol.49, pp.109-244, 1916.

M. C. Musolino, P. J. Loughlin, P. J. Sparto, and M. S. Redfern, Spectrally similar periodic and non-periodic optic flows evoke different postural sway responses, Gait & Posture, vol.23, issue.2, pp.180-188, 2006.
DOI : 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2005.02.008

H. Nyborg, A method for analysing performance in the rod-and-frame test, Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, vol.43, issue.1, pp.119-132, 1974.
DOI : 10.1037/h0053671

T. Ohlmann, Variabilité intra-individuelle et dépendance-indépendance à l'égard du champ visuel, Fontionnement cognitif et individualité, pp.185-229, 1985.

T. Ohlmann, La perception de la verticale. Variabilité interindividuelle dans la dépendance à l'égard des référentiels spatiaux, 1988.

T. Ohlmann, Evocabilité différentielle des référentiels spatiaux, posture et orientation spatiale Grenoble, Pratiques sportives et modélisation du geste, pp.215-240249, 1990.

T. Ohlmann, Processus vicariants et théorie neutraliste de l'évolution : une nécessaire convergence, Universel et Différentiel en Psychologie, pp.77-105, 1995.

T. Ohlmann and F. Brenet, Les bases posturales de la dépendance-indépendance à l'égard du champ visuel, Psychologie et Education, vol.2, pp.35-73, 1990.

T. Ohlmann, N. Chassouant, T. Cian, C. Mendelsohn, and P. , Incidence des perturbations gravito-inertielles et visuelles sur l'effet-Aubert visuel. Ref Type: Personal Communication Ohlmann, 1984.

T. Ohlmann and C. Marendaz, Vicarious processes involved in selection/control of frames of reference and spatial aspects of field dependence-independence, Field dependence-independence: Cognitive style across the life Span, pp.105-129, 1991.

K. S. Oie, T. Kiemel, and J. J. Jeka, Human multisensory fusion of vision and touch: detecting non-linearity with small changes in the sensory environment, Neuroscience Letters, vol.315, issue.3, pp.113-116, 2001.
DOI : 10.1016/S0304-3940(01)02348-5

K. S. Oie, T. Kiemel, and J. J. Jeka, Multisensory fusion: simultaneous re-weighting of vision and touch for the control of human posture, Cognitive Brain Research, vol.14, issue.1, pp.164-176, 2002.
DOI : 10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00071-X

P. K. Oltman, A PORTABLE ROD-AND-FRAME APPARATUS, Perceptual and Motor Skills, vol.1950, issue.2, pp.503-506, 1968.
DOI : 10.2466/pms.1968.26.2.503

I. Oruc, L. T. Maloney, and M. S. Landy, Weighted linear cue combination with possibly correlated error, Vision Research, vol.43, issue.23, pp.2451-2468, 2003.
DOI : 10.1016/S0042-6989(03)00435-8

C. C. Pagano and M. T. Turvey, The inertia tensor as a basis for the perception of limb orientation., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.21, issue.5, pp.1070-1087, 1995.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.21.5.1070

C. Pantev, A. Engelien, V. Candia, and T. Elbert, Representational Cortex in Musicians, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol.279, issue.1, pp.300-314, 2001.
DOI : 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb05740.x

R. J. Peterka, Simplifying the complexities of maintaining balance, IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Magazine, vol.22, issue.2, pp.63-68, 2003.
DOI : 10.1109/MEMB.2003.1195698

N. Pinsault and N. Vuillerme, Test???retest reliability of centre of foot pressure measures to assess postural control during unperturbed stance, Medical Engineering & Physics, vol.31, issue.2, pp.276-286, 2009.
DOI : 10.1016/j.medengphy.2008.08.003

M. Reuchlin, Processus vicariants et différences interindividuelles, Journal de Psychologie, vol.2, pp.133-145, 1978.

G. E. Riccio, E. J. Martin, and T. A. Stoffregen, The role of balance dynamics in the active perception of orientation., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.18, issue.3, pp.624-644, 1992.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.18.3.624

G. E. Riccio and T. A. Stoffregen, Gravitoinertial force versus the direction of balance in the perception and control of orientation., Psychological Review, vol.97, issue.1, pp.135-137, 1990.
DOI : 10.1037/0033-295X.97.1.135

I. Rock and J. Victor, Vision and Touch: An Experimentally Created Conflict between the Two Senses, Science, vol.143, issue.3606, pp.594-596, 1964.
DOI : 10.1126/science.143.3606.594

J. P. Roll, K. Popov, V. Gurfinkel, M. Lipshits, C. Andre-deshays et al., Sensorimotor and perceptual function of muscle proprioception in microgravity, J.Vestib.Res, vol.3, pp.259-273, 1993.

H. Schöne, On the role of gravity in human spatial orientation, Aerospace Medicine, vol.35, pp.764-772, 1964.

L. Shams, Y. Kamitani, and S. Shimojo, Illusions. What you see is what you hear, Nature, vol.408, issue.6814, p.788, 2000.
DOI : 10.1038/35048669

E. Sigman, D. R. Goodenough, and M. Flannagan, SUBJECTIVE ESTIMATES OF BODY TILT AND THE ROD-AND-FRAME TEST, Perceptual and Motor Skills, vol.47, issue.3f, pp.1051-1056, 1978.
DOI : 10.3758/BF03199516

D. Spinelli, G. Antonucci, R. Daini, and P. Zoccolotti, Local and global visual mechanisms underlying individual differences in the rod-and-frame illusion, Perception & Psychophysics, vol.54, issue.4, pp.915-920, 1995.
DOI : 10.3758/BF03206806

T. R. Stanford and B. E. Stein, Superadditivity in multisensory integration: putting the computation in context, NeuroReport, vol.18, issue.8, pp.787-792, 2007.
DOI : 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3280c1e315

B. E. Stein, W. S. Huneycutt, and M. A. Meredith, Neurons and behavior: the same rules of multisensory integration apply, Brain Research, vol.448, issue.2, pp.355-358, 1988.
DOI : 10.1016/0006-8993(88)91276-0

B. E. Stein and M. A. Meredith, The Merging of the Senses, 1994.

A. A. Stocker and E. P. Simoncelli, Noise characteristics and prior expectations in human visual speed perception, Nature Neuroscience, vol.93, issue.4, pp.578-585, 2006.
DOI : 10.1038/nn1669

T. A. Stoffregen and B. G. Bardy, On specification and the senses, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, vol.24, issue.02, pp.195-213, 2001.
DOI : 10.1017/S0140525X01003946

T. A. Stoffregen and G. E. Riccio, An ecological theory of orientation and the vestibular system., Psychological Review, vol.95, issue.1, pp.3-14, 1988.
DOI : 10.1037/0033-295X.95.1.3

J. W. Streepey, R. V. Kenyon, and E. A. Keshner, Visual motion combined with base of support width reveals variable field dependency in healthy young adults, Experimental Brain Research, vol.11, issue.1, 2007.
DOI : 10.1007/s00221-006-0677-2

M. J. Streibel and S. M. Ebenholtz, Construct validity of perceptual style: Role of stimulus size in the embedded-figures test and the rod-and-frame test, Perception & Psychophysics, vol.38, issue.2, pp.128-138, 1982.
DOI : 10.3758/BF03206212

K. V. Thilo, M. Guerraz, A. M. Bronstein, and M. A. Gresty, Percept-related changes in horizontal optokinetic nystagmus at different body orientations in space, Experimental Brain Research, vol.145, issue.2, pp.215-221, 2002.
DOI : 10.1007/s00221-002-1114-9

F. P. Tiecks, J. Planck, R. L. Haberl, and T. Brandt, Reduction in Posterior Cerebral Artery Blood Flow Velocity During Caloric Vestibular Stimulation, Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism, vol.11, pp.1379-1382, 1996.
DOI : 10.1097/00004647-199611000-00037

A. D. Van-beuzekom, W. P. Medendorp, and J. A. Van-gisbergen, The subjective vertical and the sense of self orientation during active body tilt, Vision Research, vol.41, issue.25-26, pp.3229-3242, 2001.
DOI : 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00144-4

R. Van-de-langenberg, I. Kingma, and P. J. Beek, Perception of limb orientation in the vertical plane depends on center of mass rather than inertial eigenvectors, Experimental Brain Research, vol.66, issue.4, pp.595-607, 2007.
DOI : 10.1007/s00221-007-0891-6

R. Van-de-langenberg, I. Kingma, and P. J. Beek, The perception of limb orientation depends on the center of mass., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.34, issue.3, pp.624-639, 2008.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.34.3.624

H. Van-der-kooij, R. Jacobs, B. Koopman, and H. Grootenboer, A multisensory integration model of human stance control, Biological Cybernetics, vol.80, issue.5, pp.299-308, 1999.
DOI : 10.1007/s004220050527

H. Van-der-kooij, R. Jacobs, B. Koopman, and F. Van-der-helm, An adaptive model of sensory integration in a dynamic environment applied to human stance control, Biological Cybernetics, vol.84, issue.2, pp.103-115, 2001.
DOI : 10.1007/s004220000196

N. Vuillerme, V. Nougier, and J. M. Prieur, Can vision compensate for a lower limbs muscular fatigue for controlling posture in humans?, Neuroscience Letters, vol.308, issue.2, pp.103-106, 2001.
DOI : 10.1016/S0304-3940(01)01987-5

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00193907

N. Vuillerme, V. Nougier, and N. Teasdale, Effects of a reaction time task on postural control in humans, Neuroscience Letters, vol.291, issue.2, pp.77-80, 2000.
DOI : 10.1016/S0304-3940(00)01374-4

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00193913

M. T. Wallace, M. A. Meredith, and B. E. Stein, Multisensory integration in the superior colliculus of the alert cat, J.Neurophysiol, vol.80, pp.1006-1010, 1998.

M. T. Wallace, L. K. Wilkinson, and B. E. Stein, Representation and integration of multiple sensory inputs in primate superior colliculus, J.Neurophysiol, vol.76, pp.1246-1266, 1996.

P. Wenderoth and H. Beh, Component Analysis of Orientation Illusions, Perception, vol.26, issue.210, pp.57-75, 1977.
DOI : 10.1068/p060057

H. A. Witkin, Perception of body position and of the position of the visual field., Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, vol.63, issue.7, pp.1-46, 1949.
DOI : 10.1037/h0093613

H. A. Witkin, INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN EASE OF PERCEPTION OF EMBEDDED FIGURES*, Journal of Personality, vol.38, issue.7, pp.1-15, 1950.
DOI : 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1950.tb01084.x

H. A. Witkin, Perception of the upright when the direction of the force acting on the body is changed., Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol.40, issue.1, pp.93-106, 1950.
DOI : 10.1037/h0055162

H. A. Witkin, Further studies of perception of the upright when the direction of the force acting on the body is changed., Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol.43, issue.1, pp.9-20, 1952.
DOI : 10.1037/h0059252

H. A. Witkin and S. E. Asch, Studies in space orientation. IV. Further experiments on perception of the upright with displaced visual fields., Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol.38, issue.6, pp.762-782, 1948.
DOI : 10.1037/h0053671

H. A. Witkin and S. E. Asch, Studies in space orientation. III. Perception of the upright in the absence of a visual field., Journal of Experimental Psychology, vol.38, issue.5, pp.603-614, 1948.
DOI : 10.1037/h0055372

H. A. Witkin and D. R. Goodenough, Cognitive styles: essence and origins. Field dependence and field independence, Psychol.Issues, pp.1-141, 1981.

H. A. Witkin, H. B. Lewis, M. Hertzman, K. Machover, B. P. Weissner et al., Personality through Perception, 1954.

L. Yardley, Contribution of somatosensory information to perception of the visual vertical with body tilt and rotating visual field, Perception & Psychophysics, vol.46, issue.2, pp.131-134, 1990.
DOI : 10.3758/BF03207079

L. R. Young, M. Shelhamer, and S. Modestino, M.I.T./Canadian vestibular experiments on the Spacelab-1 mission: 2. Visual vestibular tilt interaction in weightlessness, Experimental Brain Research, vol.64, issue.2, pp.299-307, 1986.
DOI : 10.1007/BF00237747

URL : https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/in2p3-00277323

P. Zoccolotti, G. Antonucci, and D. Spinelli, The gap between rod and frame influences the rod-and-frame effect with small and large inducing displays, Perception & Psychophysics, vol.79, issue.1, pp.14-19, 1993.
DOI : 10.3758/BF03206933

P. Zoccolotti and P. K. Oltman, Field Dependence and Lateralization of Verbal and Configurational Processing, Cortex, vol.14, issue.2, pp.155-163, 1978.
DOI : 10.1016/S0010-9452(78)80040-9

L. H. Zupan, R. J. Peterka, and D. M. Merfeld, Neural processing of gravitoinertial cues in humans. I. Influence of the semicircular canals following post-rotatory tilt, J.Neurophysiol, vol.84, 2000.